Knowledge (XXG)

Phonological development

Source 📝

30:
unlike between printed words, no spaces occur between spoken words. Thus if an infant hears the sound sequence “thisisacup,” they have to learn to segment this stream into the distinct units “this”, “is”, “a”, and “cup.” Once "cup" is able to be extracted from the speech stream, the child has to assign a meaning to this word. Furthermore, the child has to be able to distinguish the sequence “cup” from “cub” in order to learn that these are two distinct words with different meanings. Finally, the child has to learn to produce these words. The acquisition of native language
885:. The infant's tongue fills the entire mouth, thus reducing the range of movement. As the facial skeleton grows, the range for movement increases, which probably contributes to the increased variety of sounds infants start to produce. Development of muscles and sensory receptors also gives infants more control over sound production. The limited movement possible by the infant jaw and mouth might be responsible for the typical consonant-vowel (CV) alternation in 161:
sounds in a language (such as prevoiced /d/ in Spanish) occur often and infants can learn them from mere exposure to them in the speech they hear. All of this occurs before infants are aware of the meaning of any of the words they are exposed to, and therefore the phenomenon of statistical learning has been used to argue for the fact that infants can learn sound contrasts without meaning being attached to them.
896: 904: 280: 525:”. At 20 months of age, when presented with three familiar objects (e.g., a ball, a bottle and a cup) and one unfamiliar object (e.g., an egg piercer), children are able to conclude that in the request “Can I have the zib,” zib must refer to the unfamiliar object, i.e., the egg piercer, even if they have never heard that 599:/fol/ and /fir/ would be liked by a puppet whose favorite sound is /f/. 4-year-olds are less successful at this task if the onset of the syllable contains a consonant cluster, such as /fr/ or /fl/. Liberman et al. found that no 4-year-olds and only 17% of 5-year-olds were able to tap out the number of 438:
to the first word children also produce “protowords”, i.e., invented words that are used consistently to express specific meanings, but that are not real words in the children's target language. Around 12–14 months of age children produce their first word. Infants close to one year of age are able to
160:
These results show that at the age of 6 months infants are sensitive to how often certain sounds occur in the language they are exposed to and they can learn which cues are important to pay attention to from these differences in frequency of occurrence. In natural language exposure this means typical
85:
Infants up to 10–12 months can distinguish not only native sounds but also nonnative contrasts. Older children and adults lose the ability to discriminate some nonnative contrasts. Thus, it seems that exposure to one's native language causes the perceptual system to be restructured. The restructuring
520:
Of course, the reason why children need to learn the sound distinctions of their language is because then they also have to learn the meaning associated with those different sounds. Young children have a remarkable ability to learn meanings for the words they extract from the speech they are exposed
123:
With increasing exposure to the ambient language, infants learn not to pay attention to sound distinctions that are not meaningful in their native language, e.g., two acoustically different versions of the vowel /i/ that simply differ because of inter-speaker variability. By 6 months of age infants
840:
A study by Gathercole and Baddeley (1989) showed the importance of sound for early word meaning. They tested the phonological memory of 4- and 5-year-old children, i.e., how well these children were able to remember a sequence of unfamiliar sounds. They found that children with better phonological
632:
placement, such as ‘HOT dog’ vs. ‘hot DOG’ until around 12 years of age. Children in a study by Vogel and Raimy (2002) were asked to show which of two pictures (i.e., a dog or a sausage) was being named. Children younger than 12 years generally preferred the compound reading (i.e., the sausage) to
29:
Sound is at the beginning of language learning. Children have to learn to distinguish different sounds and to segment the speech stream they are exposed to into units – eventually meaningful units – in order to acquire words and sentences. One reason that speech segmentation is challenging is that
607:
until school age. Another explanation is that individual sounds do not easily translate into beats, which makes clapping individual phonemes a much more difficult task than clapping syllables. One reason why phoneme awareness gets much better once children start school is because learning to read
249:
in their native language. Their perceptual system has been tuned to the contrasts relevant in their native language. As for word comprehension, Fenson et al. (1994) tested 10-11-month-old children's comprehension vocabulary size and found a range from 11 words to 154 words. At this age, children
196:
pattern, which is the most common stress pattern in the English language, but they were not able to segment out words that follow a weak-strong pattern. In the sequence ‘guitar is’ these infants thus heard ‘taris’ as the word-unit because it follows a strong-weak pattern. The process that allows
831:
The size of the production vocabulary ranges from about 50 to 550 words at the age of 2 years. Influences on the rate of word learning, and thus on the wide range of vocabulary sizes of children of the same age, include the amount of speech children are exposed to by their caregivers as well as
69:
of speech). For example, the sounds /b/ and /p/ differ in the amount of breathiness that follows the opening of the lips. Using a computer generated continuum in breathiness between /b/ and /p/, Eimas et al. (1971) showed that English-learning infants paid more attention to differences near the
832:
differences in how rich the vocabulary in the speech a child hears is. Children also seem to build up their vocabulary faster if the speech they hear is related to their focus of attention more often. This would be the case if a caregiver talks about a ball the child is currently looking at.
503:
like these already soon after birth, only now are they able to distinguish them if they are presented to them as meaningful words rather than just a sequence of sounds. Children are also able to detect mispronunciations such as ‘vaby’ for ‘baby’. Recognition has been found to be poorer for
469:
Children are able to distinguish newly learned ‘words’ associated with objects if they are not similar-sounding, such as ‘lif’ and ‘neem’. They cannot distinguish similar-sounding newly learned words such as ‘bih’ and ‘dih’, however. So, while children at this age are able to distinguish
451:
At the age of 1, children only just begin to speak, and their utterances are not adult-like yet at all. Children's perceptual abilities are still developing, too. In fact, both production and perception abilities continue to develop well into the school years, with the perception of some
928:
begin to function. The limbic system is known to be involved in the expression of emotion, and cooing in infants is associated with a feeling of contentedness. Further development of the limbic system might be responsible for the onset of laughter around 16 weeks of age. The
283: 287: 286: 282: 281: 288: 34:
begins in the womb and isn't completely adult-like until the teenage years. Perceptual abilities (such as being able to segment “thisisacup” into four individual word units) usually precede production and thus aid the development of speech production.
633:
the phrasal reading (the dog). The authors concluded from this that children start out with a lexical bias, i.e., they prefer to interpret phrases like these as single words, and the ability to override this bias develops until late in childhood.
263:
Even though children do not produce their first words until they are approximately 12 months old, the ability to produce speech sounds starts to develop at a much younger age. Stark (1980) distinguishes five stages of early speech development:
477:
at a purely phonological level, if the discrimination task is paired with word meaning, the additional cognitive load required by learning the word meanings leaves them unable to spend the extra effort on distinguishing the similar phonology.
529:
before. Children as young as 15 months can complete this task successfully if the experiment is conducted with fewer objects. This task shows that children aged 15 to 20 months can assign meaning to a new word after only a single exposure.
696:
Children's production vocabulary size at this age is typically around 50 words, although there is great variation in vocabulary size among children in the same age group, with a range between 0 and 160 words for the majority of children.
114:
Babies prefer to hear their own name to similar-sounding words. It is possible that they have associated the meaning “me” with their name, although it is also possible that they simply recognize the form because of its high frequency.
285: 486:
Children's comprehension vocabulary size ranges from about 92 to 321 words. The production vocabulary size at this age is typically around 50 words. This shows that comprehension vocabulary grows faster than production vocabulary.
372:(e.g., ). At this stage, infants’ productions resemble speech much more closely in timing and vocal behaviors than at earlier stages. Starting around 6 months babies also show an influence of the ambient language in their 705:
Children's productions become more consistent around the age of 18 months. When their words differ from adult forms, these differences are more systematic than before. These systematic transformations are referred to as
70:
boundary between /b/ and /p/ than to equal-sized differences within the /b/-category or within the /p/-category. Their measure, monitoring infant sucking-rate, became a major experimental method for studying infant
534:
is a necessary ability for children to acquire the number of words they have to learn during the first few years of life: Children acquire an average of nine words per day between 18 months and 6 years of age.
153:/t/ continuum (similar to the /d/ - /t/ distinction in Spanish) with the majority of the tokens occurring near the endpoints of the continuum, i.e., showing extreme prevoicing versus long voice onset times ( 551:. Phonological awareness does continue to develop until the first years of school. For example, only about half of the 4- and 5-year-olds tested by Liberman et al. (1974) were able to tap out the number of 841:
memory also had larger vocabularies at both ages. Moreover, phonological memory at age 4 predicted the children's vocabulary at age 5, even with earlier vocabulary and nonverbal intelligence factored out.
889:
and it has even been suggested that the predominance of CV syllables in the languages of the world might evolutionarily have been caused by this limited range of movements of the human vocal organs.
603:(individual sounds) in a word. 70% of 6-year-olds were able to do so. This might mean that children are aware of syllables as units of speech early on, while they don't show awareness of individual 924:. Neurological development of higher brain structures coincides with certain developments in infants’ vocalizations. For example, the onset of cooing at 6 to 8 weeks happens as some areas of the 48:
Children do not utter their first words until they are about 1 year old, but already at birth they can tell some utterances in their native language from utterances in languages with different
142:
Infants are able to extract meaningful distinctions in the language they are exposed to from statistical properties of that language. For example, if English-learning infants are exposed to a
98:
to adult-directed speech. Whereas 1-month-olds only exhibit this preference if the full speech signal is played to them, 4-month-old infants prefer infant-directed speech even when just the
595:
is made up of the vowel and all following consonants. For example, the onset in the word ‘dog’ is /d/ and the rime is /og/. Children at 3–4 years of age were able to tell that the
213:
While children generally don't understand the meaning of most single words yet, they understand the meaning of certain phrases they hear a lot, such as “Stop it,” or “Come here.”
284: 1001: 81:
Fig. 1. Sucking rate for 20 ms VOT change across category boundary (left), 20 ms VOT change within category (middle), without VOT change (right). After Eimas et al.(1971).
2140: 2048: 1805: 1522: 1144: 1066: 168:
features of the ambient language to break the speech stream they are exposed to into meaningful units, e.g., they are better able to distinguish sounds that occur in
688:
that get put together online when uttering them. This is suggested by the fact that infants at this age may produce the same sounds differently in different words.
321:, with the first back consonants (e.g., , ) being produced around 2–3 months, and front consonants (e.g., , , ) starting to appear around 6 months of age. As for 865:
The developmental changes in infants’ vocalizations over the first year of life are influenced by physical developments during that time. Physical growth of the
106:
information in the speech directed at them. By 4 months, finally, infants have learned which features they have to pay attention to at the suprasegmental level.
1778:
Werker, J. F.; C. T. Fennel, K. M. Corcoran, C. L. Stager (2002). "Infants' ability to learn phonetically similar words: Effects of age and vocabulary size".
1593: 680:
Infants usually produce their first word around 12 –14 months of age. First words are simple in structure and contain the same sounds that were used in late
305:
Infants produce cooing sounds when they are content. Cooing is often triggered by social interaction with caregivers and resembles the production of vowels.
137: 869:, brain development, and development of neurological structures responsible for vocalization are factors for the development of infants’ vocal productions. 157:) they are better at discriminating these sounds than infants who are exposed primarily to tokens from the center of the continuum (unimodal distribution). 2302:
Gathercole, S. E.; A. D. Baddeley (1989). "Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study".
250:
normally have not yet begun to speak and thus have no production vocabulary. So clearly, comprehension vocabulary develops before production vocabulary.
380:
sounds different depending on which languages they hear. For example, French learning 9-10 month-olds have been found to produce a bigger proportion of
521:
to, i.e., to map meaning onto the sounds. Often children already associate a meaning with a new word after only one exposure. This is referred to as “
1495:
Jusczyk, P. W.; A. D. Friederici, J. M. I. Wessels, V. Y. Svenkerud (1993). "Infants' sensitivity to the soundpatterns of native language words".
272:
These earliest vocalizations include crying and vegetative sounds such as breathing, sucking or sneezing. For these vegetative sounds, infants’
1573:
Stark, R. E. (1980). "Stages of speech development in the first year of life". In Yeni-Komshian, G. H.; J. F. Kavanagh, C. A. Ferguson (eds.).
1023:
Mehler, J.; P. Jusczyk, G. Lambertz, N. Halsted, J. Bertoncini, C. Amiel-Tison (1988). "A precursor of language acquisition in young infants".
977: 1158:
Werker, J. F.; R. C. Tees (1984). "Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life".
337:
length or tension at the end of the vocalization, or both. At 3 to 9 months infants don't seem to be able to control these movements yet.
2364:
MacNeilage, P. F.; B. L. Davis (2001). "Motor mechanisms in speech ontogeny: phylogenetic, neurobiological and linguistic implications".
1536:
Werker, J F; R C Tees (1984). "Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life".
325:
in early infant utterances, infants between 3 and 9 months of age produce primarily flat, falling and rising-falling contours. Rising
313:
Infants produce a variety of vowel- and consonant-like sounds that they combine into increasingly longer sequences. The production of
1452:
Fenson, L.; P. S. Dale; J. S. Reznick; E. Bates; D. J. Thal; S. J. Pethick (1994). "Variability in early communicative development".
276:
vibrate and air passes through their vocal apparatus, thus familiarizing infants with processes involved in later speech production.
1759: 2011:
Liberman, I. Y.; D. Shankweiler, F. W. Fischer, B. Carter (1974). "Explicit syllable and phoneme segmentation in the young child".
899:
Fig. 3. Infant vocal tract: H = hard palate, S = soft palate, T = tongue, J = jaw, E = epiglottis, G = glottis; After Vihman (1996)
907:
Fig. 4. Adult vocal tract: H = hard palate, S = soft palate, T = tongue, J = jaw, E = epiglottis, G = glottis; After Vihman (1996)
1301:
Maye, J.; J. F. Werker, L. Gerken (2002). "Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination".
1009: 667: 649: 188:
At 7.5 months English-learning infants have been shown to be able to segment words from speech that show a strong-weak (i.e.,
78: 892:
The differences between the vocal tract of infants and adults can be seen in figure 3 (infants) and figure 4 (adults) below.
2225:
Walley, A. C. (1993). "The role of vocabulary development in children's spoken word recognition and segmentation ability".
2417: 555:
in multisyllabic words, but 90% of the 6-year-olds were able to do so. Most 3- to 4-year-olds are able to break simple
1950:
Markman, E. M.; J. L. Wasow, M. B. Hansen (2003). "Use of the mutual exclusivity assumption by young word learners".
504:
mispronounced than for correctly pronounced words. This suggests that infants’ representations of familiar words are
718:
in adult Jamaican Creole: “yellow yellow” = “very yellow” ). Some common phonological processes are listed below.
2154:
Vogel, I.; E. Raimy (2002). "The acquisition of compound vs. phrasal stress: the role of prosodic constituents".
2097:
Bryant, P. E.; L. Bradley, M. Maclean, J. Crossland (1989). "Nursery rhymes, phonological skills and reading".
1819:
Swingley, D.; R. N. Asley (2000). "Spoken word recognition and lexical representation in very young children".
431: 128:, such as an /i/ spoken by a male versus a female speaker, as members of the same phonological category /i/. 2020: 1591:
Kent, R. D.; A. D. Murray (1982). "Acoustic features of infant vocalic utterances at 3, 6, and 9 months".
1545: 1167: 663: 659: 645: 621: 246: 150: 125: 66: 2134: 2042: 1799: 1516: 1138: 1060: 850: 685: 509: 499:’, even if they are phonologically similar, e.g. ‘bih’ and ‘dih’. While infants are able to distinguish 453: 230: 198: 165: 103: 49: 1647:
Oller, D. K. (1986). "Metaphonology and infant vocalizations". In Lindblom, B.; R. Zetterstrom (eds.).
757:: production of two identical syllables based on one of the target word syllables, e.g., for ‘bottle’ 245:
Infants now can no longer discriminate most nonnative sound contrasts that fall within the same sound
1602: 1092: 731: 629: 427: 193: 177: 169: 154: 2025: 1550: 1344:
Karzon, R. G. (1985). "Discrimination of polysyllabic sequences by one- to four-month-old infants".
1172: 801: 143: 933:, finally, which develops later than the abovementioned structures may be necessary for canonical 508:
very precise. This result has also been taken to suggest that infants move from a word-based to a
180:
patterns in the speech they are exposed and they have learned that these patterns are meaningful.
2412: 2389: 2179: 2122: 1975: 1924: 1854: 1626: 1469: 1410: 1326: 1126: 1048: 1993:
Carey, S. (1978). "The child as a word learner". In Halle, M.; J. Bresnan, G. A. Miller (eds.).
1907:
Mervis, C. B.; J. Bertrand (1994). "Acquisition of the novel name-nameless category principle".
766:: a target word consonant takes on features of another target word consonant, e.g., for ‘duck’ 1083:
Eimas, P. D.; E. R. Siqueland, P. Jusczyk, J. Vigorito (1971). "Speech perception in infants".
124:
have learned to treat acoustically different sounds that are representations of the same sound
2381: 2346: 2284: 2171: 2114: 2079: 1967: 1932: 1889: 1846: 1755: 1728: 1693: 1618: 1477: 1402: 1361: 1318: 1274:
Kuhl, P. K. (1983). "Perception of auditory equivalence classes for speech in early infancy".
1256: 1118: 1040: 983: 973: 790: 762: 684:. The lexical items they produce are probably stored as whole words rather than as individual 381: 147: 71: 2062:
Treiman, R. (1985). "Onsets and rimes as units of spoken syllables: Evidence from children".
2373: 2338: 2311: 2276: 2234: 2163: 2106: 2071: 2030: 1959: 1916: 1881: 1836: 1828: 1787: 1720: 1683: 1675: 1610: 1555: 1504: 1461: 1392: 1353: 1310: 1283: 1246: 1238: 1207: 1177: 1108: 1100: 1032: 816: 620:
patterns in their native language at 7–8 months, they are not able to reliably distinguish
102:
are played. This shows that between 1 and 4 months of age, infants improve in tracking the
65:
Infants as young as 1 month perceive some speech sounds as speech categories (they display
1841: 1113: 921: 786: 1606: 1198:; P. K. Kuhl (1987). "Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech". 1096: 2208: 1748: 1688: 1663: 1251: 1242: 1226: 805: 576: 568: 384: 2377: 1963: 1832: 1559: 1314: 1287: 1181: 2406: 2315: 2075: 2034: 1357: 1211: 1036: 925: 753: 715: 592: 572: 440: 326: 322: 202: 99: 2393: 2126: 1979: 1630: 1414: 1130: 1052: 2183: 1858: 1195: 930: 548: 531: 522: 474: 234: 226: 1330: 895: 1104: 201:
cues in speech input to learn about language structure has been termed “prosodic
1872:
Dollaghan, C. (1985). "Child meets word: "Fast mapping" in preschool children".
882: 878: 866: 710:
processes”, and often resemble processes that are typically common in the adult
608:
provides a visual aid as how to break up words into their smaller constituents.
395:
being influenced by the language being acquired has been called babbling drift.
334: 273: 1679: 903: 2167: 2110: 1791: 917: 596: 526: 987: 2280: 2267:
Hoff, E.; L. Naigles (2002). "How children use input to acquire a lexicon".
820: 745: 711: 707: 584: 556: 544: 505: 419: 365: 318: 222: 95: 31: 23: 2385: 2288: 2238: 2175: 1971: 1850: 1732: 1697: 1508: 1430:
Signal to Syntax: Bootstrapping from Speech to Grammar in Early Acquisition
1406: 1397: 1380: 1322: 1260: 2350: 2118: 2083: 1936: 1893: 1885: 1622: 1481: 1365: 1122: 1044: 22:
refers to how children learn to organize sounds into meaning or language (
2342: 934: 886: 854: 773:: omission of a consonant in a target word cluster, e.g., for ‘cracker’ 734: 681: 655: 580: 564: 552: 500: 496: 471: 435: 423: 392: 388: 377: 373: 361: 357: 173: 1928: 1473: 604: 600: 330: 189: 1724: 1614: 625: 617: 1920: 1465: 237:, i.e., which combinations of sounds are possible in the language. 221:
Infants can distinguish native from nonnative language input using
902: 894: 588: 560: 415: 391:
than English learning infants of the same age. This phenomenon of
369: 317:
sounds (already in the first 2 months) precedes the production of
314: 278: 76: 443:
in addition to flat, falling, and rising-falling pitch contours.
176:. This means that at 6 months infants have some knowledge of the 1381:"The beginning of word segmentation in English-learning infants" 881:
are smaller, and initially also shaped differently from adults’
547:
awareness, i.e., they are interested in word play, rhyming, and
456:
features not being fully developed until about 12 years of age.
77: 1227:"Infants' recognition of the sound patterns of their own names" 399:
10-14 months: Nonreduplicated babbling (or variegated babbling)
292:
A 14-week-old infant cooing as she interacts with a caregiver (
2210:
Phonological Development. The Origins of Language in the Child
1664:"VOT in the babbling of French- and English-learning infants" 2256:. Columbus, OH: The Ohio State University, PhD Dissertation. 2254:
The Phonology and Phonetics of Jamaican Creole Reduplication
777:
Segment substitution processes (into the early school years)
1454:
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development
341:
6-10 months: Reduplicated babbling (or canonical babbling)
2329:
Sander, E. K. (1972). "When are speech sounds learned?".
1002:"Babies learn words before birth | Humans | Science News" 86:
reflects the system of contrasts in the native language.
2006: 2004: 857:
words continues to improve throughout middle childhood.
233:
cues. They seem to have learned their native language's
853:. Their ability to produce complex sound sequences and 495:
At 18–20 months infants can distinguish newly learned ‘
1662:
Whalen, D. H.; A. G. Levitt, L. M. Goldstein (2007).
861:
Biological foundations of infants’ speech development
426:
strings. At this stage, infants also produce various
364:
that are repeated in reduplicated series of the same
1586: 1584: 1078: 1076: 654:. For an introductory guide on IPA symbols, see 512:-based phonological system around 18 months of age. 1773: 1771: 1750:
Language and Context: The Acquisition of Pragmatics
1225:Mandel, D. R.; P. W. Jusczyk, D. B. Pisoni (1995). 916:Crying and vegetative sounds are controlled by the 2207: 1747: 1711:Brown, R. (1958). "How shall a thing be called?". 1379:Jusczyk, P. W.; D. M. Houston, M. Newsome (1999). 447:Development once speech sets in (1 year and older) 164:At 6 months, infants are also able to make use of 387:(which exist in French but not English) in their 668:IPA § Brackets and transcription delimiters 333:pressure during the vocalization or to increase 1651:. New York, NY: Stockton Press. pp. 21–35. 1577:. New York, NY: Academic Press. pp. 73–92. 434:patterns. During this transitional period from 16:Acquisition of language skills during childhood 2201: 2199: 2197: 2195: 2193: 1997:. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. pp. 264–293. 1447: 1445: 1443: 1441: 1439: 8: 2139:: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list ( 2047:: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list ( 1995:Linguistics Theory and Psychological Reality 1804:: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list ( 1642: 1640: 1594:Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 1521:: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list ( 1143:: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list ( 1065:: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list ( 963: 961: 959: 957: 955: 953: 951: 949: 138:Statistical learning in language acquisition 937:, which start around 6 to 9 months of age. 972:. Boston, MA: Wadsworth/Cengage Learning. 229:patterns alone, i.e., without the help of 39:Prelinguistic development (birth – 1 year) 2024: 1840: 1687: 1549: 1396: 1250: 1171: 1112: 748:in the target word, e.g., for ‘because’ 2064:Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 2013:Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 1346:Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 737:in the target word, e.g., for ‘banana’ 543:At 2 years, infants show first signs of 2331:Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 945: 722:Whole word processes (until age 3 or 4) 2132: 2040: 1874:Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 1797: 1514: 1136: 1058: 259:Stages of pre-speech vocal development 1575:Child Phonology. Volume 1: Production 7: 658:. For the distinction between , 94:At four months infants still prefer 849:Children produce mostly adult-like 329:would require the infants to raise 1243:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00517.x 268:0-6 weeks: Reflexive vocalizations 14: 1428:Morgan, J. L.; K. Demuth (1996). 920:, which matures earlier than the 26:) during their stages of growth. 1754:. New York, NY: Academic Press. 2366:Current Opinion in Neurobiology 1538:Infant Behavior and Development 1460:(5): 1–173, discussion 174–85. 1276:Infant Behavior and Development 1200:Infant Behavior and Development 1160:Infant Behavior and Development 650:International Phonetic Alphabet 301:6-16 weeks: Cooing and laughter 2304:Journal of Memory and Language 1842:11858/00-001M-0000-000E-E627-8 1497:Journal of Memory and Language 1114:11858/00-001M-0000-002B-0DB3-1 714:of the world's languages (cf. 414:Infants now combine different 360:contains consonant-vowel (CV) 1: 2378:10.1016/S0959-4388(01)00271-9 1964:10.1016/S0010-0285(03)00034-3 1833:10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00081-0 1560:10.1016/S0163-6383(84)80022-3 1315:10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00157-3 1288:10.1016/S0163-6383(83)80036-8 1182:10.1016/S0163-6383(84)80022-3 2316:10.1016/0749-596X(89)90044-2 2076:10.1016/0022-0965(85)90034-7 2035:10.1016/0022-0965(74)90101-5 1358:10.1016/0022-0965(85)90044-X 1212:10.1016/0163-6383(87)90017-8 1105:10.1126/science.171.3968.303 1037:10.1016/0010-0277(88)90035-2 567:up into their constituents ( 771:Consonant cluster reduction 616:Although children perceive 2434: 1680:10.1016/j.wocn.2006.10.001 1649:Precursors of Early Speech 135: 2168:10.1017/S0305000902005020 2156:Journal of Child Language 2111:10.1017/S0305000900010485 2099:Journal of Child Language 1792:10.1207/s15327078in0301_1 587:preceding the syllable's 2214:. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. 793:sound, e.g., for ‘key’ 744:: omission of the final 742:Final consonant deletion 20:Phonological development 2281:10.1111/1467-8624.00415 1432:. Mahwah, NJ: Earlbaum. 646:phonetic transcriptions 309:16-30 weeks: Vocal play 2239:10.1006/drev.1993.1015 2206:Vihman, M. M. (1996). 1509:10.1006/jmla.1993.1022 1398:10.1006/cogp.1999.0716 908: 900: 823:, e.g., for ‘rabbit’ 728:Weak syllable deletion 643:This section contains 297: 96:infant-directed speech 82: 67:categorical perception 61:Categorical perception 1886:10.1044/jshr.2803.454 1231:Psychological Science 906: 898: 291: 80: 2418:Language acquisition 2343:10.1044/jshd.3701.55 2227:Developmental Review 1952:Cognitive Psychology 1713:Psychological Review 1668:Journal of Phonetics 1385:Cognitive Psychology 970:Language development 873:Infants’ vocal tract 628:that differ only in 583:consists of all the 155:bimodal distribution 132:Statistical learning 2252:Gooden, S. (2003). 1607:1982ASAJ...72..353K 1097:1971Sci...171..303E 1006:www.sciencenews.org 968:Erika Hoff (2009). 808:, e.g., for ‘sea’ 1746:Bates, E. (1976). 912:The nervous system 909: 901: 298: 83: 2269:Child Development 1909:Child Development 1091:(3968): 303–306. 979:978-0-495-50171-8 819:is replaced by a 804:is replaced by a 789:is replaced by a 763:Consonant harmony 730:: omission of an 563:-consonant (CVC) 289: 72:speech perception 2425: 2398: 2397: 2361: 2355: 2354: 2326: 2320: 2319: 2299: 2293: 2292: 2264: 2258: 2257: 2249: 2243: 2242: 2222: 2216: 2215: 2213: 2203: 2188: 2187: 2151: 2145: 2144: 2138: 2130: 2094: 2088: 2087: 2059: 2053: 2052: 2046: 2038: 2028: 2008: 1999: 1998: 1990: 1984: 1983: 1947: 1941: 1940: 1915:(6): 1646–1662. 1904: 1898: 1897: 1869: 1863: 1862: 1844: 1816: 1810: 1809: 1803: 1795: 1775: 1766: 1765: 1753: 1743: 1737: 1736: 1725:10.1037/h0041727 1708: 1702: 1701: 1691: 1659: 1653: 1652: 1644: 1635: 1634: 1615:10.1121/1.388089 1588: 1579: 1578: 1570: 1564: 1563: 1553: 1533: 1527: 1526: 1520: 1512: 1492: 1486: 1485: 1449: 1434: 1433: 1425: 1419: 1418: 1400: 1391:(3–4): 159–207. 1376: 1370: 1369: 1341: 1335: 1334: 1298: 1292: 1291: 1282:(2–3): 263–285. 1271: 1265: 1264: 1254: 1222: 1216: 1215: 1192: 1186: 1185: 1175: 1155: 1149: 1148: 1142: 1134: 1116: 1080: 1071: 1070: 1064: 1056: 1020: 1014: 1013: 1008:. Archived from 998: 992: 991: 965: 665: 661: 411: 410: 406: 376:, i.e., babies’ 353: 352: 348: 290: 2433: 2432: 2428: 2427: 2426: 2424: 2423: 2422: 2403: 2402: 2401: 2363: 2362: 2358: 2328: 2327: 2323: 2301: 2300: 2296: 2266: 2265: 2261: 2251: 2250: 2246: 2224: 2223: 2219: 2205: 2204: 2191: 2153: 2152: 2148: 2131: 2096: 2095: 2091: 2061: 2060: 2056: 2039: 2026:10.1.1.602.5825 2010: 2009: 2002: 1992: 1991: 1987: 1949: 1948: 1944: 1921:10.2307/1131285 1906: 1905: 1901: 1871: 1870: 1866: 1818: 1817: 1813: 1796: 1777: 1776: 1769: 1762: 1745: 1744: 1740: 1710: 1709: 1705: 1661: 1660: 1656: 1646: 1645: 1638: 1590: 1589: 1582: 1572: 1571: 1567: 1551:10.1.1.537.6695 1535: 1534: 1530: 1513: 1494: 1493: 1489: 1466:10.2307/1166093 1451: 1450: 1437: 1427: 1426: 1422: 1378: 1377: 1373: 1343: 1342: 1338: 1300: 1299: 1295: 1273: 1272: 1268: 1224: 1223: 1219: 1194: 1193: 1189: 1173:10.1.1.537.6695 1157: 1156: 1152: 1135: 1082: 1081: 1074: 1057: 1022: 1021: 1017: 1000: 999: 995: 980: 967: 966: 947: 943: 914: 875: 863: 847: 838: 829: 779: 724: 703: 694: 678: 673: 672: 671: 639: 614: 541: 518: 493: 484: 467: 462: 449: 439:produce rising 412: 408: 404: 402: 401: 354: 350: 346: 344: 343: 311: 303: 279: 270: 261: 256: 243: 219: 211: 197:infants to use 186: 172:vs. unstressed 140: 134: 121: 112: 92: 63: 58: 46: 41: 17: 12: 11: 5: 2431: 2429: 2421: 2420: 2415: 2405: 2404: 2400: 2399: 2372:(6): 696–700. 2356: 2321: 2310:(2): 200–213. 2294: 2275:(2): 418–433. 2259: 2244: 2233:(3): 286–350. 2217: 2189: 2162:(2): 225–250. 2146: 2105:(2): 407–428. 2089: 2070:(1): 181–191. 2054: 2019:(2): 201–212. 2000: 1985: 1958:(3): 241–275. 1942: 1899: 1880:(3): 449–454. 1864: 1827:(2): 147–166. 1811: 1767: 1760: 1738: 1703: 1674:(3): 341–352. 1654: 1636: 1601:(2): 353–363. 1580: 1565: 1528: 1503:(3): 402–420. 1487: 1435: 1420: 1371: 1352:(2): 326–342. 1336: 1309:(3): 101–111. 1293: 1266: 1237:(5): 314–317. 1217: 1206:(3): 279–293. 1187: 1150: 1072: 1031:(2): 143–178. 1015: 1012:on 2013-08-26. 993: 978: 944: 942: 939: 913: 910: 874: 871: 862: 859: 846: 843: 837: 834: 828: 825: 783:Velar fronting 778: 775: 723: 720: 702: 699: 693: 690: 677: 674: 666:⟩, see 642: 641: 640: 638: 635: 613: 610: 540: 537: 517: 514: 492: 489: 483: 480: 466: 463: 461: 458: 448: 445: 441:pitch contours 400: 397: 342: 339: 327:pitch contours 323:pitch contours 310: 307: 302: 299: 269: 266: 260: 257: 255: 252: 242: 239: 218: 215: 210: 207: 185: 182: 136:Main article: 133: 130: 120: 117: 111: 108: 104:suprasegmental 100:pitch contours 91: 88: 62: 59: 57: 54: 45: 42: 40: 37: 15: 13: 10: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 2430: 2419: 2416: 2414: 2411: 2410: 2408: 2395: 2391: 2387: 2383: 2379: 2375: 2371: 2367: 2360: 2357: 2352: 2348: 2344: 2340: 2336: 2332: 2325: 2322: 2317: 2313: 2309: 2305: 2298: 2295: 2290: 2286: 2282: 2278: 2274: 2270: 2263: 2260: 2255: 2248: 2245: 2240: 2236: 2232: 2228: 2221: 2218: 2212: 2211: 2202: 2200: 2198: 2196: 2194: 2190: 2185: 2181: 2177: 2173: 2169: 2165: 2161: 2157: 2150: 2147: 2142: 2136: 2128: 2124: 2120: 2116: 2112: 2108: 2104: 2100: 2093: 2090: 2085: 2081: 2077: 2073: 2069: 2065: 2058: 2055: 2050: 2044: 2036: 2032: 2027: 2022: 2018: 2014: 2007: 2005: 2001: 1996: 1989: 1986: 1981: 1977: 1973: 1969: 1965: 1961: 1957: 1953: 1946: 1943: 1938: 1934: 1930: 1926: 1922: 1918: 1914: 1910: 1903: 1900: 1895: 1891: 1887: 1883: 1879: 1875: 1868: 1865: 1860: 1856: 1852: 1848: 1843: 1838: 1834: 1830: 1826: 1822: 1815: 1812: 1807: 1801: 1793: 1789: 1785: 1781: 1774: 1772: 1768: 1763: 1761:9780120815500 1757: 1752: 1751: 1742: 1739: 1734: 1730: 1726: 1722: 1718: 1714: 1707: 1704: 1699: 1695: 1690: 1685: 1681: 1677: 1673: 1669: 1665: 1658: 1655: 1650: 1643: 1641: 1637: 1632: 1628: 1624: 1620: 1616: 1612: 1608: 1604: 1600: 1596: 1595: 1587: 1585: 1581: 1576: 1569: 1566: 1561: 1557: 1552: 1547: 1543: 1539: 1532: 1529: 1524: 1518: 1510: 1506: 1502: 1498: 1491: 1488: 1483: 1479: 1475: 1471: 1467: 1463: 1459: 1455: 1448: 1446: 1444: 1442: 1440: 1436: 1431: 1424: 1421: 1416: 1412: 1408: 1404: 1399: 1394: 1390: 1386: 1382: 1375: 1372: 1367: 1363: 1359: 1355: 1351: 1347: 1340: 1337: 1332: 1328: 1324: 1320: 1316: 1312: 1308: 1304: 1297: 1294: 1289: 1285: 1281: 1277: 1270: 1267: 1262: 1258: 1253: 1248: 1244: 1240: 1236: 1232: 1228: 1221: 1218: 1213: 1209: 1205: 1201: 1197: 1191: 1188: 1183: 1179: 1174: 1169: 1165: 1161: 1154: 1151: 1146: 1140: 1132: 1128: 1124: 1120: 1115: 1110: 1106: 1102: 1098: 1094: 1090: 1086: 1079: 1077: 1073: 1068: 1062: 1054: 1050: 1046: 1042: 1038: 1034: 1030: 1026: 1019: 1016: 1011: 1007: 1003: 997: 994: 989: 985: 981: 975: 971: 964: 962: 960: 958: 956: 954: 952: 950: 946: 940: 938: 936: 932: 927: 926:limbic system 923: 919: 911: 905: 897: 893: 890: 888: 884: 880: 872: 870: 868: 860: 858: 856: 855:multisyllabic 852: 844: 842: 835: 833: 826: 824: 822: 818: 814: 809: 807: 803: 799: 794: 792: 788: 784: 776: 774: 772: 767: 765: 764: 758: 756: 755: 754:Reduplication 749: 747: 743: 738: 736: 733: 729: 721: 719: 717: 716:reduplication 713: 709: 700: 698: 691: 689: 687: 683: 675: 669: 657: 653: 651: 647: 636: 634: 631: 627: 623: 619: 611: 609: 606: 602: 598: 594: 590: 586: 582: 578: 574: 570: 566: 562: 558: 554: 550: 549:alliterations 546: 538: 536: 533: 528: 524: 515: 513: 511: 507: 502: 498: 490: 488: 481: 479: 476: 475:minimal pairs 473: 464: 459: 457: 455: 446: 444: 442: 437: 433: 429: 425: 421: 417: 407: 398: 396: 394: 390: 386: 383: 379: 375: 371: 367: 363: 359: 356:Reduplicated 349: 340: 338: 336: 332: 328: 324: 320: 316: 308: 306: 300: 295: 277: 275: 267: 265: 258: 253: 251: 248: 240: 238: 236: 232: 228: 224: 216: 214: 208: 206: 204: 203:bootstrapping 200: 195: 191: 183: 181: 179: 175: 171: 167: 162: 158: 156: 152: 149: 145: 139: 131: 129: 127: 118: 116: 109: 107: 105: 101: 97: 89: 87: 79: 75: 73: 68: 60: 55: 53: 51: 43: 38: 36: 33: 27: 25: 21: 2369: 2365: 2359: 2337:(1): 55–63. 2334: 2330: 2324: 2307: 2303: 2297: 2272: 2268: 2262: 2253: 2247: 2230: 2226: 2220: 2209: 2159: 2155: 2149: 2135:cite journal 2102: 2098: 2092: 2067: 2063: 2057: 2043:cite journal 2016: 2012: 1994: 1988: 1955: 1951: 1945: 1912: 1908: 1902: 1877: 1873: 1867: 1824: 1820: 1814: 1800:cite journal 1783: 1779: 1749: 1741: 1719:(1): 14–21. 1716: 1712: 1706: 1671: 1667: 1657: 1648: 1598: 1592: 1574: 1568: 1541: 1537: 1531: 1517:cite journal 1500: 1496: 1490: 1457: 1453: 1429: 1423: 1388: 1384: 1374: 1349: 1345: 1339: 1306: 1302: 1296: 1279: 1275: 1269: 1234: 1230: 1220: 1203: 1199: 1190: 1163: 1159: 1153: 1139:cite journal 1088: 1084: 1061:cite journal 1028: 1024: 1018: 1010:the original 1005: 996: 969: 931:motor cortex 915: 891: 883:vocal tracts 879:vocal tracts 876: 864: 848: 839: 830: 812: 810: 797: 795: 782: 780: 770: 768: 761: 759: 752: 750: 741: 739: 727: 725: 708:phonological 704: 695: 679: 676:12-14 months 662:and ⟨ 644: 615: 545:phonological 542: 532:Fast mapping 523:fast mapping 519: 516:Fast-mapping 506:phonetically 494: 491:18-20 months 485: 472:monosyllabic 468: 450: 413: 355: 312: 304: 293: 271: 262: 244: 241:10-12 months 235:phonotactics 220: 212: 187: 163: 159: 141: 122: 113: 93: 84: 64: 47: 28: 19: 18: 1196:Fernald, A. 867:vocal tract 712:phonologies 274:vocal cords 227:phonotactic 151:unaspirated 2407:Categories 941:References 918:brain stem 732:unstressed 637:Production 624:words and 591:, and the 585:consonants 527:pseudoword 460:Perception 432:intonation 420:consonants 335:vocal fold 331:subglottal 319:consonants 294:51 seconds 254:Production 52:features. 44:Perception 2413:Phonology 2021:CiteSeerX 1821:Cognition 1546:CiteSeerX 1544:: 49–63. 1303:Cognition 1168:CiteSeerX 1166:: 49–63. 1025:Cognition 988:759925056 802:fricative 746:consonant 701:18 months 692:16 months 565:syllables 557:consonant 553:syllables 539:2–6 years 501:syllables 482:16 months 465:14 months 382:prevoiced 366:consonant 362:syllables 174:syllables 148:voiceless 144:prevoiced 32:phonology 24:phonology 2394:34697879 2386:11741020 2289:11949900 2176:12109370 2127:28419790 1980:42489580 1972:14559217 1851:10856741 1786:: 1–30. 1733:13505978 1698:19641636 1631:12186661 1415:12097435 1407:10631011 1323:11747867 1261:25152566 1131:15554065 1053:43126875 935:babbling 887:babbling 877:Infants 851:segments 798:Stopping 735:syllable 686:segments 682:babbling 660:/ / 656:Help:IPA 622:compound 618:rhythmic 612:12 years 605:phonemes 601:phonemes 597:nonwords 581:syllable 454:prosodic 436:babbling 424:syllable 393:babbling 389:babbling 378:babbling 374:babbling 358:babbling 247:category 231:prosodic 223:phonetic 217:9 months 209:8 months 199:prosodic 190:trochaic 184:7 months 170:stressed 166:prosodic 126:category 119:6 months 110:5 months 90:4 months 50:prosodic 2351:5053945 2184:9588933 2119:2760133 2084:3989458 1937:7859547 1929:1131285 1894:4046586 1859:6324150 1780:Infancy 1689:2717044 1623:7119278 1603:Bibcode 1482:7845413 1474:1166093 1366:3989467 1252:4140581 1123:5538846 1093:Bibcode 1085:Science 1045:3168420 845:7 years 836:4 years 827:2 years 813:Gliding 791:coronal 648:in the 626:phrases 575:). The 510:segment 403:)": --> 345:)": --> 146:/d/ to 56:1 month 2392:  2384:  2349:  2287:  2182:  2174:  2125:  2117:  2082:  2023:  1978:  1970:  1935:  1927:  1892:  1857:  1849:  1758:  1731:  1696:  1686:  1629:  1621:  1548:  1480:  1472:  1413:  1405:  1364:  1331:319422 1329:  1321:  1259:  1249:  1170:  1129:  1121:  1051:  1043:  986:  976:  922:cortex 817:liquid 664:  630:stress 428:stress 416:vowels 194:stress 178:stress 2390:S2CID 2180:S2CID 2123:S2CID 1976:S2CID 1925:JSTOR 1855:S2CID 1627:S2CID 1470:JSTOR 1411:S2CID 1327:S2CID 1127:S2CID 1049:S2CID 821:glide 787:velar 652:(IPA) 589:vowel 579:of a 577:onset 569:onset 561:vowel 497:words 422:into 385:stops 370:vowel 315:vowel 2382:PMID 2347:PMID 2285:PMID 2172:PMID 2141:link 2115:PMID 2080:PMID 2049:link 1968:PMID 1933:PMID 1890:PMID 1847:PMID 1806:link 1756:ISBN 1729:PMID 1694:PMID 1619:PMID 1523:link 1478:PMID 1403:PMID 1362:PMID 1319:PMID 1257:PMID 1145:link 1119:PMID 1067:link 1041:PMID 984:OCLC 974:ISBN 815:: a 806:stop 800:: a 785:: a 593:rime 573:rime 571:and 430:and 418:and 405:edit 368:and 347:edit 225:and 2374:doi 2339:doi 2312:doi 2277:doi 2235:doi 2164:doi 2107:doi 2072:doi 2031:doi 1960:doi 1917:doi 1882:doi 1837:hdl 1829:doi 1788:doi 1721:doi 1684:PMC 1676:doi 1611:doi 1556:doi 1505:doi 1462:doi 1393:doi 1354:doi 1311:doi 1284:doi 1247:PMC 1239:doi 1208:doi 1178:doi 1109:hdl 1101:doi 1089:171 1033:doi 205:”. 2409:: 2388:. 2380:. 2370:11 2368:. 2345:. 2335:37 2333:. 2308:28 2306:. 2283:. 2273:73 2271:. 2231:13 2229:. 2192:^ 2178:. 2170:. 2160:29 2158:. 2137:}} 2133:{{ 2121:. 2113:. 2103:16 2101:. 2078:. 2068:39 2066:. 2045:}} 2041:{{ 2029:. 2017:18 2015:. 2003:^ 1974:. 1966:. 1956:47 1954:. 1931:. 1923:. 1913:65 1911:. 1888:. 1878:28 1876:. 1853:. 1845:. 1835:. 1825:76 1823:. 1802:}} 1798:{{ 1782:. 1770:^ 1727:. 1717:65 1715:. 1692:. 1682:. 1672:35 1670:. 1666:. 1639:^ 1625:. 1617:. 1609:. 1599:72 1597:. 1583:^ 1554:. 1540:. 1519:}} 1515:{{ 1501:32 1499:. 1476:. 1468:. 1458:59 1456:. 1438:^ 1409:. 1401:. 1389:39 1387:. 1383:. 1360:. 1350:39 1348:. 1325:. 1317:. 1307:82 1305:. 1278:. 1255:. 1245:. 1233:. 1229:. 1204:10 1202:. 1176:. 1162:. 1141:}} 1137:{{ 1125:. 1117:. 1107:. 1099:. 1087:. 1075:^ 1063:}} 1059:{{ 1047:. 1039:. 1029:29 1027:. 1004:. 982:. 948:^ 811:- 796:- 781:- 769:- 760:- 751:- 740:- 726:- 192:) 74:. 2396:. 2376:: 2353:. 2341:: 2318:. 2314:: 2291:. 2279:: 2241:. 2237:: 2186:. 2166:: 2143:) 2129:. 2109:: 2086:. 2074:: 2051:) 2037:. 2033:: 1982:. 1962:: 1939:. 1919:: 1896:. 1884:: 1861:. 1839:: 1831:: 1808:) 1794:. 1790:: 1784:3 1764:. 1735:. 1723:: 1700:. 1678:: 1633:. 1613:: 1605:: 1562:. 1558:: 1542:7 1525:) 1511:. 1507:: 1484:. 1464:: 1417:. 1395:: 1368:. 1356:: 1333:. 1313:: 1290:. 1286:: 1280:6 1263:. 1241:: 1235:6 1214:. 1210:: 1184:. 1180:: 1164:7 1147:) 1133:. 1111:: 1103:: 1095:: 1069:) 1055:. 1035:: 990:. 706:“ 670:. 559:- 409:] 351:] 296:)

Index

phonology
phonology
prosodic
categorical perception
speech perception

infant-directed speech
pitch contours
suprasegmental
category
Statistical learning in language acquisition
prevoiced
voiceless
unaspirated
bimodal distribution
prosodic
stressed
syllables
stress
trochaic
stress
prosodic
bootstrapping
phonetic
phonotactic
prosodic
phonotactics
category
vocal cords
vowel

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.