Knowledge

Rhyme-as-reason effect

Source 📝

89:, this heuristic references his famous line, "Beauty is truth, truth beauty - that is all / Ye know on earth, and all ye need to know," a rhyming aphorism celebrated for its poetic beauty and philosophical depth. This heuristic suggests that people may partially base their assessments of a statement's truthfulness on its aesthetic attributes. 111:
The fluency heuristic is defined as the tendency to attribute higher value to objects or phrases that are more easily retrieved or processed. According to this heuristic, the perceived value of a phrase is linked to how quickly and effortlessly it is processed. Rhymed sayings typically exhibit higher
150:
it does not facilitate "transfer-appropriate" information to long-term memory, making it difficult for children to grasp the sentences' meanings. Thus, the "rhyme-as-reason" effect may result in superficial processing that is inadequate for semantic comprehension in children's cognitive development.
149:
In early childhood education, the phonological resemblance found in nursery rhymes may lead young toddlers to focus more on phonological elements than on semantic ones, negatively affecting short-term memory retention. This may be because phonetic processing creates fleeting memory traces or because
145:
Research shows that people strongly prefer rhyming slogans over their non-rhyming equivalents, finding them more endearing, unique, memorable, and convincing. This makes rhymes particularly effective in advertising. However, the quality of the rhymes is crucial for determining their trustworthiness.
92:
Experimental results show that participants consistently rated rhyming aphorisms as more agreeable and truthful than non-rhyming ones, even when the content was identical or the rhyming content lacked logical validity. This effect is strongest when the poetic qualities, such as fluency from rhyming,
64:
The seminal study, "Birds of a Feather Flock Conjointly (?): Rhyme as Reason in Aphorisms," investigates the influence of rhyme on the creation and perception of aphorisms. The study concludes that rhyming aphorisms are more memorable and persuasive, acting as a heuristic that enhances their impact.
132:
Additionally, the "rhyme-as-reason effect" is influenced by the level of attention allocated to content. When participants in experiments were explicitly instructed to distinguish between poetic structure and semantic content, the perceived truthfulness of rhyming sayings was significantly reduced.
128:
Despite the prevalence of well-known rhyming aphorisms (e.g., "A friend in need is a friend indeed"), critics argue that participants may favor these not for their rhyming properties, but because they subconsciously associate them with the accumulated wisdom of the ages. Questionnaires that focused
115:
People do not always make decisions based on rational analysis or declarative knowledge. Instead, the ease of processing can result in more positive evaluations of aphorisms. Stimuli that are processed with difficulty tend to feel psychologically distant and are perceived in a more abstract manner.
68:
Research on the "chiastic structure," a type of linguistic structure that rearranges phrasing in an aesthetically pleasing way, demonstrates its effectiveness in increasing the perceived accuracy of statements. This higher perceived truthfulness is likely due to the memorable and coherent nature of
76:
Moreover, people often rely on heuristics, such as "reputable sources make true assertions" or "familiar sayings are believable," especially when lacking the expertise or motivation to thoroughly evaluate a message. This reliance is typically subconscious, suggesting that under certain conditions,
72:
In the study "A Reason to Rhyme: Phonological and Semantic Influences on Lexical Access," participants exposed to rhyming primes in a verbal sentence completion task responded faster than those exposed to non-rhyming primes. This indicates that rhyme enhances lexical access, making rhymed sayings
136:
Furthermore, the frequency and recency of exposure to relevant stimuli strongly correlate with retrieval fluency, linking back to fluency heuristics. This suggests that the effect may be more about enhanced processing fluency, which arises from repeated exposure or environmental factors such as
119:
For example, cities with names that are processed smoothly are perceived as closer than those with names that are processed less fluently. This preference for fluency explains why rhymed sayings are often judged as more truthful and agreeable: their ease of cognitive processing makes them more
42:
In experiments, participants evaluated variations of sayings that either rhymed or did not rhyme. Those that rhymed were consistently judged as more truthful, even when the meaning was controlled for. For instance, the rhyming saying "What sobriety conceals, alcohol reveals" was rated as more
157:
These findings underscore the impact of language in persuasive communication and suggest that linguistic aesthetics can shape our perception of the world. By understanding the rhyme-as-reason effect, we can better appreciate how language influences thought and behavior in various contexts.
96:
Therefore, it can be inferred that the presence of rhyme within an aphorism's poetic structure acts as a cue to deeper meaning. People prefer rhymes for their "pleasurable aesthetics," as the rhyme creates a sense of unity and coherence, increasing the aphorism's appeal, repeatability, and
153:
In the legal context, rhymes can enhance jurors' retention and implementation of attorneys' instructions, potentially biasing the outcomes. This manipulation shows how rhyme can influence decision-making even in critical settings like a courtroom.
146:
To ensure that customers accept claims automatically and without deep analysis, marketing messages should be as fluent as possible. This highlights the significant role aesthetics play in everyday life, influencing our choices and perceptions.
43:
accurate on average than its non-rhyming counterpart, "What sobriety conceals, alcohol unmasks," across different groups of subjects (each group assessed the accuracy of only one version of the statement).
137:
fluency manipulations, rather than the presence of rhyme itself. These factors create greater familiarity and ease of recall, contributing to the perceived truthfulness of the statements.
85:
The Keats heuristics illustrate how poetic structure influences the perception of a line of words, highlighting a broader concept of how aesthetics impact our judgments. Named after
46:
This effect may be explained by the Keats heuristic, which suggests that people assess a statement's truth based on its aesthetic qualities. Another explanation is the
97:
memorability. This cognitive bias is often explained by fluency heuristics, where the ease of processing a rhyming statement enhances its perceived truthfulness.
129:
on the content features of social advertising slogans found no significant difference in the perceived credibility of rhymes versus non-rhymes.
316:
Kara-Yakoubian, Mane; Walker, Alexander C.; Sharpinskyi, Konstantyn; Assadourian, Garni; Fugelsang, Jonathan A.; Harris, Randy A. (June 2022).
643:"Effects of Fluency on Psychological Distance and Mental Construal (or Why New York Is a Large City, but New York Is a Civilized Jungle)" 920: 226:
McGlone, M. S.; J. Tofighbakhsh (2000). "Birds of a feather flock conjointly (?): rhyme as reason in aphorisms".
112:
fluency, making them easier to retrieve and process, which leads to the assumption that they have greater value.
133:
Similar reductions in the effect were observed when participants had pre-existing opinions about the aphorisms.
791:"Dissociation of processes in belief: Source recollection, statement familiarity, and the illusion of truth" 274:
McGlone, M. S.; J. Tofighbakhsh (1999). "The Keats heuristic: Rhyme as reason in aphorism interpretation".
176: 318:"Beauty and truth, truth and beauty: Chiastic structure increases the subjective accuracy of statements" 206:
The Power of Computational Thinking: Games, Magic and Puzzles to Help You Become a Computational Thinker
59: 876: 642: 838:"Nursery rhymes and prose passages: Preschoolers' liking and short-term retention of story events" 818: 771: 678: 506: 449: 353: 251: 790: 585: 468: 373: 896: 857: 837: 810: 763: 724: 670: 662: 623: 615: 566: 558: 498: 441: 402: 394: 345: 337: 243: 106: 47: 888: 849: 802: 755: 714: 654: 605: 597: 548: 540: 488: 480: 433: 386: 329: 283: 235: 743: 529:"Fluency heuristic: A model of how the mind exploits a by-product of information retrieval" 610: 553: 702: 116:
Conversely, the most appealing choices are often those that are the simplest to process.
50:, which posits that statements are preferred due to their ease of cognitive processing. 28: 892: 287: 914: 775: 658: 528: 357: 682: 510: 255: 822: 806: 601: 484: 390: 853: 527:
Hertwig, Ralph; Herzog, Stefan M.; Schooler, Lael J.; Reimer, Torsten (2008).
190: 86: 900: 861: 814: 767: 759: 666: 619: 562: 502: 445: 398: 341: 239: 191:"The Rhyme-as-Reason Effect: Why Rhyming Makes Your Message More Persuasive" 728: 674: 627: 570: 406: 374:"A reason to rhyme: Phonological and semantic influences on lexical access" 349: 247: 77:
people may equate the ease of rhyming with the truthfulness of a message.
493: 32: 875:
Morris, C. Donald; Bransford, John D.; Franks, Jeffery J. (1977-10-01).
719: 453: 421: 333: 93:
closely align with the perceived truthfulness of the semantic content.
836:
Hayes, Donald S.; Chemelski, Bruce E.; Palmer, Melvin (January 1982).
544: 437: 533:
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
469:"The halo effect: Evidence for unconscious alteration of judgments" 379:
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
789:
Begg, Ian Maynard; Anas, Ann; Farinacci, Suzanne (December 1992).
36: 317: 877:"Levels of processing versus transfer appropriate processing" 177:"Timex and Beowulf and a copywriting secret you should know" 641:
Alter, Adam L.; Oppenheimer, Daniel M. (February 2008).
742:
Jacoby, Larry L.; Kelley, Colleen M. (September 1987).
703:"Rhyme as reason in commercial and social advertising" 467:
Nisbett, Richard E.; Wilson, Timothy D. (April 1977).
701:
Filkuková, Petra; Klempe, Sven Hroar (October 2013).
35:
are perceived as more accurate or truthful when they
744:"Unconscious Influences of Memory for a Prior Event" 372: 584:Schooler, Lael J.; Hertwig, Ralph (July 2005). 881:Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 8: 473:Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 204:McOwan, Peter William; Curzon, Paul (2017). 795:Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 322:Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 748:Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 371:Rapp, David N.; Samuel, Arthur G. (2002). 718: 609: 586:"How forgetting aids heuristic inference" 552: 492: 422:"Review of The Psychology of Attitudes" 167: 696: 694: 692: 522: 520: 305:. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. 7: 269: 267: 265: 221: 219: 217: 215: 707:Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 14: 659:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02062.x 73:more memorable and acceptable. 611:11858/00-001M-0000-0025-838B-B 554:11858/00-001M-0000-0024-FC25-9 420:Chakravarti, Dipankar (1997). 1: 893:10.1016/S0022-5371(77)80016-9 426:Journal of Marketing Research 288:10.1016/s0304-422x(99)00003-0 807:10.1037/0096-3445.121.4.446 602:10.1037/0033-295X.112.3.610 937: 485:10.1037/0022-3514.35.4.250 391:10.1037/0278-7393.28.3.564 104: 57: 854:10.1037/0012-1649.18.1.49 301:Kahneman, Daniel (2011). 842:Developmental Psychology 760:10.1177/0146167287133003 120:appealing and credible. 303:Thinking, Fast and Slow 240:10.1111/1467-9280.00282 141:Real world implications 175:Marsh, Robert (2017). 25:Eaton–Rosen phenomenon 21:rhyme-as-reason effect 647:Psychological Science 228:Psychological Science 69:chiastic structures. 60:Illusory truth effect 590:Psychological Review 54:Studies and theories 23:, also known as the 720:10.1111/sjop.12069 334:10.1037/cep0000277 101:Fluency heuristics 107:Fluency heuristic 48:fluency heuristic 31:where sayings or 928: 921:Cognitive biases 905: 904: 872: 866: 865: 833: 827: 826: 786: 780: 779: 739: 733: 732: 722: 698: 687: 686: 638: 632: 631: 613: 581: 575: 574: 556: 545:10.1037/a0013025 539:(5): 1191–1206. 524: 515: 514: 496: 464: 458: 457: 417: 411: 410: 376: 368: 362: 361: 313: 307: 306: 298: 292: 291: 271: 260: 259: 223: 210: 209: 201: 195: 194: 187: 181: 180: 172: 81:Keats heuristics 936: 935: 931: 930: 929: 927: 926: 925: 911: 910: 909: 908: 874: 873: 869: 835: 834: 830: 788: 787: 783: 741: 740: 736: 700: 699: 690: 640: 639: 635: 583: 582: 578: 526: 525: 518: 466: 465: 461: 438:10.2307/3151869 419: 418: 414: 370: 369: 365: 315: 314: 310: 300: 299: 295: 273: 272: 263: 225: 224: 213: 203: 202: 198: 189: 188: 184: 174: 173: 169: 164: 143: 126: 109: 103: 83: 62: 56: 17: 12: 11: 5: 934: 932: 924: 923: 913: 912: 907: 906: 887:(5): 519–533. 867: 828: 801:(4): 446–458. 781: 754:(3): 314–336. 734: 713:(5): 423–431. 688: 653:(2): 161–167. 633: 596:(3): 610–628. 576: 516: 479:(4): 250–256. 459: 432:(2): 298–303. 412: 385:(3): 564–571. 363: 328:(2): 144–155. 308: 293: 282:(4): 235–244. 261: 234:(5): 424–428. 211: 196: 182: 166: 165: 163: 160: 142: 139: 125: 122: 105:Main article: 102: 99: 82: 79: 55: 52: 29:cognitive bias 16:Cognitive bias 15: 13: 10: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 933: 922: 919: 918: 916: 902: 898: 894: 890: 886: 882: 878: 871: 868: 863: 859: 855: 851: 847: 843: 839: 832: 829: 824: 820: 816: 812: 808: 804: 800: 796: 792: 785: 782: 777: 773: 769: 765: 761: 757: 753: 749: 745: 738: 735: 730: 726: 721: 716: 712: 708: 704: 697: 695: 693: 689: 684: 680: 676: 672: 668: 664: 660: 656: 652: 648: 644: 637: 634: 629: 625: 621: 617: 612: 607: 603: 599: 595: 591: 587: 580: 577: 572: 568: 564: 560: 555: 550: 546: 542: 538: 534: 530: 523: 521: 517: 512: 508: 504: 500: 495: 494:2027.42/92158 490: 486: 482: 478: 474: 470: 463: 460: 455: 451: 447: 443: 439: 435: 431: 427: 423: 416: 413: 408: 404: 400: 396: 392: 388: 384: 380: 375: 367: 364: 359: 355: 351: 347: 343: 339: 335: 331: 327: 323: 319: 312: 309: 304: 297: 294: 289: 285: 281: 277: 270: 268: 266: 262: 257: 253: 249: 245: 241: 237: 233: 229: 222: 220: 218: 216: 212: 207: 200: 197: 192: 186: 183: 178: 171: 168: 161: 159: 155: 151: 147: 140: 138: 134: 130: 123: 121: 117: 113: 108: 100: 98: 94: 90: 88: 80: 78: 74: 70: 66: 61: 53: 51: 49: 44: 40: 38: 34: 30: 26: 22: 884: 880: 870: 848:(1): 49–56. 845: 841: 831: 798: 794: 784: 751: 747: 737: 710: 706: 650: 646: 636: 593: 589: 579: 536: 532: 476: 472: 462: 429: 425: 415: 382: 378: 366: 325: 321: 311: 302: 296: 279: 275: 231: 227: 205: 199: 185: 170: 156: 152: 148: 144: 135: 131: 127: 118: 114: 110: 95: 91: 84: 75: 71: 67: 63: 45: 41: 24: 20: 18: 162:References 124:Criticisms 87:John Keats 58:See also: 901:0022-5371 862:1939-0599 815:1939-2222 776:145729162 768:0146-1672 667:0956-7976 620:1939-1471 563:1939-1285 503:1939-1315 446:0022-2437 399:1939-1285 358:247361524 342:1878-7290 33:aphorisms 915:Category 729:23841497 683:15403377 675:18271864 628:16060753 571:18763900 511:17867385 407:12018508 350:35266782 256:15967239 248:11228916 454:3151869 276:Poetics 193:. 2019. 27:, is a 899:  860:  823:229079 821:  813:  774:  766:  727:  681:  673:  665:  626:  618:  569:  561:  509:  501:  452:  444:  405:  397:  356:  348:  340:  254:  246:  819:S2CID 772:S2CID 679:S2CID 507:S2CID 450:JSTOR 354:S2CID 252:S2CID 37:rhyme 897:ISSN 858:ISSN 811:ISSN 764:ISSN 725:PMID 671:PMID 663:ISSN 624:PMID 616:ISSN 567:PMID 559:ISSN 499:ISSN 442:ISSN 403:PMID 395:ISSN 346:PMID 338:ISSN 244:PMID 19:The 889:doi 850:doi 803:doi 799:121 756:doi 715:doi 655:doi 606:hdl 598:doi 594:112 549:hdl 541:doi 489:hdl 481:doi 434:doi 387:doi 330:doi 284:doi 236:doi 917:: 895:. 885:16 883:. 879:. 856:. 846:18 844:. 840:. 817:. 809:. 797:. 793:. 770:. 762:. 752:13 750:. 746:. 723:. 711:54 709:. 705:. 691:^ 677:. 669:. 661:. 651:19 649:. 645:. 622:. 614:. 604:. 592:. 588:. 565:. 557:. 547:. 537:34 535:. 531:. 519:^ 505:. 497:. 487:. 477:35 475:. 471:. 448:. 440:. 430:34 428:. 424:. 401:. 393:. 383:28 381:. 377:. 352:. 344:. 336:. 326:76 324:. 320:. 280:26 278:. 264:^ 250:. 242:. 232:11 230:. 214:^ 39:. 903:. 891:: 864:. 852:: 825:. 805:: 778:. 758:: 731:. 717:: 685:. 657:: 630:. 608:: 600:: 573:. 551:: 543:: 513:. 491:: 483:: 456:. 436:: 409:. 389:: 360:. 332:: 290:. 286:: 258:. 238:: 208:. 179:.

Index

cognitive bias
aphorisms
rhyme
fluency heuristic
Illusory truth effect
John Keats
Fluency heuristic
"Timex and Beowulf and a copywriting secret you should know"
"The Rhyme-as-Reason Effect: Why Rhyming Makes Your Message More Persuasive"




doi
10.1111/1467-9280.00282
PMID
11228916
S2CID
15967239



doi
10.1016/s0304-422x(99)00003-0
"Beauty and truth, truth and beauty: Chiastic structure increases the subjective accuracy of statements"
doi
10.1037/cep0000277
ISSN
1878-7290
PMID

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.