Knowledge (XXG)

Attitude change

Source πŸ“

337:
People who have high levels of self-esteem, who are postulated to possess abilities to reduce dissonance by focusing on positive aspects of the self, have also been found to prefer modifying cognitions, such as attitudes and beliefs, over self-affirmation. A simple example of cognitive dissonance resulting in attitude change would be when a heavy smoker learns that his sister died young from lung cancer due to heavy smoking as well, this individual experiences conflicting cognitions: the desire to smoke, and the knowledge that smoking could lead to death and a desire not to die. In order to reduce dissonance, this smoker could change his behavior (i.e. stop smoking), change his attitude about smoking (i.e. smoking is harmful), or retain his original attitude about smoking and modify his new cognition to be consistent with the first one--"I also work out so smoking won't be harmful to me". Thus, attitude change is achieved when individuals experience feelings of uneasiness or guilt due to cognitive dissonance, and actively reduce the dissonance through changing their attitude, beliefs, or behavior relating in order to achieve consistency with the inconsistent cognitions.
169:, attitude accessibility, issue involvement, and message/source features. Attitudes that are central to one's being are highly resistant to change while others that are less fixed may change with new experiences or information. A new attitude (e.g. to time-keeping or absenteeism or quality) may challenge existing beliefs or norms so creating a feeling of psychological discomfort known as cognitive dissonance. It is difficult to measure attitude change since attitudes may only be inferred and there might be significant divergence between those publicly declared and privately held. Self-efficacy is a perception of one's own human agency; in other words, it is the perception of our own ability to deal with a situation. It is an important variable in emotional appeal messages because it dictates a person's ability to deal with both the emotion and the situation. For example, if a person is not self-efficacious about their ability to impact the global environment, they are not likely to change their attitude or behaviour about global warming. 391:
Comparison processes can be enhanced when prior evaluations, associated information, or both are accessible. People will simply construct a current judgment based on the new information or adjust the prior judgment when they are not able to retrieve the information from prior messages. The impact this comparative process can have on people's attitude change is mediated by changes in the strength of new information perceived by receivers. The effects of comparison on judgment change were mediated by changes in the perceived strength of the information. These findings above have wide range of applications in social marketing, political communication, and health promotion. For example, designing an advertisement that is counteractive against an existing attitude towards a behavior or policy is perhaps most effective if the advertisement uses the same format, characters, or music of ads associated with the initial attitudes.
107:
options, the line that is the same length as a sample and were asked to give the answer out loud. Unbeknown to the participants, Asch had placed a number of confederates to deliberately give the wrong answer before the participant. The results showed that 75% of responses were in line with majority influence and were the same answers the confederates picked. Variations in the experiments showed that compliance rates increased as the number of confederates increased, and the plateau was reached with around 15 confederates. The likelihood of compliance dropped with minority opposition, even if only one confederate gave the correct answer. The basis for compliance is founded on the fundamental idea that people want to be accurate and right.
306:. In the elaboration likelihood model, cognitive processing is the central route and affective/emotion processing is often associated with the peripheral route. The central route pertains to an elaborate cognitive processing of information while the peripheral route relies on cues or feelings. The ELM suggests that true attitude change only happens through the central processing route that incorporates both cognitive and affective components as opposed to the more heuristics-based peripheral route. This suggests that motivation through emotion alone will not result in an attitude change. 179:, the challenge for researchers is measuring emotion and subsequent impacts on attitude. Since we cannot see into the brain, various models and measurement tools have been constructed to obtain emotion and attitude information. Measures may include the use of physiological cues like facial expressions, vocal changes, and other body rate measures. For instance, fear is associated with raised eyebrows, increased heart rate and increased body tension. Other methods include concept or network mapping, and using primes or word cues. 329:
motivated to reduce dissonance which can be achieved through changing their attitudes and beliefs. Cooper & Fazio's (1984) have also added that cognitive dissonance does not arise from any simple cognitive inconsistency, but rather results from freely chosen behavior that may bring about negative consequences. These negative consequences may be threats to the consistency, stability, predictability, competence, moral goodness of the self-concept, or violation of general self-integrity.
383:
with prior messages, regardless of whether they can recall the prior messages after they reach a conclusion. This comparative processing mechanism is built on "information-integration theory" and "social judgement theory". Both of these theories have served to model people's attitude change in judging the new information while they have not adequately explained the influential factors that motivate people to integrate the information.
225:
the strength or logic of the argument. Motivation can be determined by many factors, such as how personally relevant the topic is, and cognitive ability can be determined by how knowledgeable an individual is on the message topic, or whether or not there is a distraction in the room. Individuals who receive a message through systematic processing usually internalize the message, resulting in a longer and more stable attitude change.
374:
in prior attitudes. Individuals need to resolve the conflict between their own behaviors and the subsequent beliefs. However, people usually align themselves with their attitudes and beliefs instead of their behaviors. More importantly, this process of resolving people's cognitive conflicts that emerges cuts across both self-perception and dissonance even when the associated effect may only be strong in changing prior attitudes
80: 158:, and attitude change. Much of attitude research has emphasised the importance of affective or emotion components. Emotion works hand-in-hand with the cognitive process, or the way we think, about an issue or situation. Emotional appeals are commonly found in advertising, health campaigns and political messages. Recent examples include no-smoking health campaigns (see 162:) and political campaigns emphasizing the fear of terrorism. Attitude change based on emotions can be seen vividly in serial killers who are faced with major stress. There is considerable empirical support for the idea that emotions in the form of fear arousal, empathy, or a positive mood can enhance attitude change under certain conditions. 403: 175:, otherwise known as intuition or the prediction of emotion, also impacts attitude change. Research suggests that predicting emotions is an important component of decision making, in addition to the cognitive processes. How we feel about an outcome may override purely cognitive rationales. In terms of research 370:
behaviors. The influence of past behavior on current attitudes is stable when little information conflicts with the behavior. Alternatively, people's attitudes may lean more radically toward the prior behavior if the conflict makes it difficult to ignore, and forces them to rationalize their past behavior.
241:
Heuristic processing occurs when individuals have low motivation and/or low cognitive ability to process a message. Instead of focusing on the argument of the message, recipients using heuristic processing focus on more readily accessible information and other unrelated cues, such as the authority or
228:
According to the heuristic-systematic model of information processing, people are motivated to use systematic processing when they want to achieve a "desired level of confidence" in their judgments. There are factors that have been found to increase the use of systematic processing; these factors are
90:
refers to a change in behavior based on consequences, such as an individual's hopes to gain rewards or avoid punishment from another group or person. The individual does not necessarily experience changes in beliefs or evaluations of an attitude object, but rather is influenced by the social outcomes
386:
More recent work in the area of persuasion has further explored this "comparative processing" from the perspective of focusing on comparing between different sets of information on one single issue or object instead of simply making comparisons among different issues or objects. As previous research
373:
Attitudes are often restructured at the time people are asked to report them. As a result, inconsistencies between the information that enters into the reconstruction and the original attitudes can produce changes in prior attitudes, whereas consistency between these elements often elicits stability
278:
plays a role in attitude change in situations where there are superior-inferior relationships. We are more likely to become obedient to authorities when the authority's expertise is perceived as high and when we anticipate receiving rewards. A famous study that constitutes the difference in attitude
245:
For example, people are more likely to grant favors if reasons are provided. A study shows that when people said, "Excuse me, I have five pages to xerox. May I use the copier?" they received a positive response of 60%. The statement, "Excuse me, I have five pages to xerox. I am in a rush. May I use
118:
explains one's change of beliefs and affect in order to be similar to someone one admires or likes. In this case, the individual adopts the new attitude, not due to the specific content of the attitude object, but because it is associated with the desired relationship. Often, children's attitudes on
268:
is the means by which we utilize other people's behaviors in order to form our own beliefs. Our attitudes toward following the majority change when a situation appears uncertain or ambiguous to us, when the source is an expert, or when the source is similar to us. In a study conducted by Sherif, he
209:
The heuristic-systematic model of information processing describes two depths in the processing of attitude change, systematic processing and heuristic processing. In this model information is either processed in a high-involvement and high-effort systematic way, or information is processed through
130:
refers to the change in beliefs and affect when one finds the content of the attitude to be intrinsically rewarding, and thus leads to actual change in beliefs or evaluations of an attitude object. The new attitude or behavior is consistent with the individual's value system, and tends to be merged
382:
Human judgment is comparative in nature. Departing from identifying people's need to justify their own beliefs in the context of their own behaviors, psychologists also believe that people have the need to carefully evaluate new messages on the basis of whether these messages support or contradict
224:
Systematic processing occurs when individuals are motivated and have high cognition to process a message. Individuals using systematic processing are motivated to pay attention and have the cognitive ability to think deeply about a message; they are persuaded by the content of the message, such as
336:
has been shown to reduce dissonance, however it is not always the mode of choice when trying to reduce dissonance. When multiple routes are available, it has been found that people prefer to reduce dissonance by directly altering their attitudes and behaviors rather than through self-affirmation.
1194:
Erber, M. W., Hodges, S. D., & Wilson, T. D. (1995). Attitude strength, attitude stability, and the effects of analyzing reasons. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences. Ohio State University series on attitudes and persuasion (Vol. 4, pp.
359:
Mild fear appeals lead to more attitude change than strong fear appeals. Propagandists had often used fear appeals. Hoveland's evidence about the effect of such appeals suggested that a source should be cautious in using fear appeals, because strong fear messages may interfere with the intended
328:
or uneasiness when two linked cognitions are inconsistent, such as when there are two conflicting attitudes about a topic, or inconsistencies between one's attitude and behavior on a certain topic. The basic idea of the Cognitive Dissonance Theory relating to attitude change, is that people are
286:
Liking has shown that if one likes another party, one is more inclined to carry out a favor. The attitude changes are based on whether an individual likes an idea or person, and if he or she does not like the other party, he/she may not carry out the favor or do so out of obligation. Liking can
390:
When people compare different sets of information on one single issue or object, the effect of people's effort to compare new information with prior information seemed to correlate with the perceived strength of the new, strong information when considered jointly with the initial information.
232:
Systematic processing has been shown to be beneficial in social influence settings. Systematic reasoning has been shown to be successful in producing more valid solutions during group discussions and greater solution accuracy. Shestowsky's (1998) research in dyad discussions revealed that the
106:
asked groups of students to participate in a "vision test". In reality, all but one of the participants were confederates of the experimenter, and the study was really about how the remaining student would react to the confederates' behavior. Participants were asked to pick, out of three line
369:
The process of how people change their own attitudes has been studied for years. Belief rationalization has been recognized as an important aspect to understand this process. The stability of people's past attitudes can be influenced if they hold beliefs that are inconsistent with their own
1204:
Judd, C. M., & Brauer, M. (1995). Repetition and evaluative extremity. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences. Ohio State University series on attitudes and persuasion (Vol. 4, pp. 43-71). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
615:
Breckler, S. J., & Wiggins, E. C. (1992). On defining attitude and attitude theory: Once more with feeling. In A. R. Pratkanis, S. J. Breckler, & A. G. Greenwald (Eds.) Attitude Structure and Function (pp. 407–427). Hillsdale, NJ:
229:
associated with either decreasing an individual's actual confidence or increasing an individual's perceived confidence. These factors may include framing persuasive messages in an unexpected manner; self-relevancy of the message.
777:
Chaiken, S., Liberman, A. & Eagly, A. H. (1989). Heuristic and systematic information processing within and beyond the persuasion context. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh. (Eds.), Unintended thought, pp. 212-252. New York:
269:
discovered the power of crowds when he worked with experimenters who looked up in the middle of New York City. As the number of the precipitating group increased, the percentage of passers-by who looked up increased as well.
242:
attractiveness of the speaker. Individuals who process a message through heuristic processing do not internalize the message, and thus any attitude change resulting from the persuasive message is temporary and unstable.
46:
and cognitive components. It has been suggested that the inter-structural composition of an associative network can be altered by the activation of a single node. Thus, by activating an affective or emotional node,
138:
is based on internalization of attitude change. This model states that the behavior towards some object is a function of an individual's intent, which is a function of one's overall attitude towards the action.
187:
Many dual process models are used to explain the affective (emotional) and cognitive processing and interpretations of messages, as well as the different depths of attitude change. These include the
670:
Leventhal, H. A. (1970) Findings and theory in the study of fear communications.In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 120-186). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
272:
Reciprocity is returning a favor. People are more likely to return a favor if they have a positive attitude towards the other party. Reciprocities also develop interdependence and societal bonds.
517:
Eagly, A., & Chaiken, S. (1995). Attitude strength, attitude structure and resistance to change. In R. Petty and J. Kosnik (Eds.), Attitude Strength. (pp. 413–432). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
1335:
Sherif, M., & Hovland, C. I. (1961). Placement of items on controversial issues. In M. Sherif & C. Hovland (Eds.), Social judgment (pp. 99–126). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
387:
demonstrated, analyzing information on one target product may trigger less impact of comparative information than comparing this product with the same product under competing brands.
303: 204: 188: 283:, where people changed their attitude to "shocking their partner" more when they followed authorities whereas the subjects themselves would have not done so otherwise. 714:
Loewenstein, G. (2007). Affect regulation and affective forecasting. In Gross, J. J. (Ed.) Handbook of Emotion Regulation (pp. 180–203). New York: Guilford.
896:
Shestowsky, D; Wegener, DT; Fabrigar, LR. (1998). "Need for cognition and interpersonal influence: individual differences in impact on dyadic decision".
568:
Asch, S. E. (1956). Studies of independence and conformity: A minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs, 70 (Whole no. 416).
212: 83:
One of the pairs of cards used in the experiment. The card on the left has the reference line and the one on the right shows the three comparison lines.
131:
with the individual's existing values and beliefs. Therefore, behaviors adopted through internalization are due to the content of the attitude object.
1093:
Gibbons, FX; Eggleston, TJ; Benthin, AC (1997). "Cognitive reactions to smoking relapse: the reciprocal relation between dissonance and self-esteem".
955:
Albarracin, D., Johnson, B. T., & Zanna, M. P. (20050. Then handbook of attitudes. Mahwah, N.J: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
751:
Shavelson, R. J.; Stanton, G. C. (1975). "Construct validation: Methodology and application to three measures of cognitive structure".
348:
and his band of persuasion researchers learned a great deal during World War 2 and later at Yale about the process of attitude change.
1421:
Muthukrishnan, A. V.; Pham, M. T.; Mungale, A. (2001). "Does greater amount of information always bolster attitudinal resistance?".
1003: 791:
Chaiken, S. (1980). "Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source versus message cues in persuasion".
30:
towards some object. They are not stable, and because of the communication and behavior of other people, are subject to change by
91:
of adopting a change in behavior. The individual is also often aware that he or she is being urged to respond in a certain way.
1058:
Stone, J; Wiegand, AW; Cooper, J; Aronson, E (1997). "When exemplification fails: hypocrisy and the motive for self-integrity".
1471: 302:
The elaboration likelihood model is similar in concept to and shares many ideas with other dual processing models, such as the
216:. For example, emotions are affect-based heuristics, in which feelings and gut-feeling reactions are often used as shortcuts. 287:
influence one's opinions through factors such as physical attractiveness, similarities, compliments, contact and cooperation.
1476: 431: 297: 192: 127: 115: 95: 68: 64: 233:
individual in the dyad who had high motivation and high need in cognition had the greater impact on group decisions.
966: 1139:"Maintenance and decay of past behavior influences: Anchoring attitudes on beliefs following inconsistent actions" 1461: 42:
occursβ€”when two attitudes or attitude and behavior conflict. Attitudes and attitude objects are functions of
1466: 534:
Kelman, H.C. (1958). "Compliance, identification, and internalization: Three processes of attitude change".
135: 87: 60: 426: 352:
High-credibility sources lead to more attitude change immediately following the communication act, but a
172: 19: 1036:
Aronson E. 1992. The return of the repressed:Dissonance theory makes a comeback. Psychol.Inq. 3:303–11
315: 39: 625:
Davis, E. E. (1965). Attitude change: A review and bibliography of selected research. Paris: Unesco.
254: 159: 119:
race, or their political party affiliations are adopted from their parents' attitudes and beliefs.
1481: 1438: 1283: 1240: 878: 808: 697: 551: 481: 280: 103: 1018:
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
861:
Smith, SM; Petty, RE. (1996). "Message framing and persuasion: a message processing analysis".
578:
Cialdini, Robert B.; Goldstein, Noah J. (2004). "SOCIAL INFLUENCE: Compliance and Conformity".
1376: 1318: 1275: 1232: 1215:
Mussweiler, T (2003). "Comparison processes in social judgment: mechanisms and consequences".
1168: 1110: 1075: 999: 843: 595: 473: 408: 1430: 1403: 1366: 1358: 1310: 1267: 1224: 1158: 1150: 1102: 1067: 989: 981: 905: 870: 835: 800: 760: 733: 689: 587: 543: 463: 333: 155: 31: 1271: 1027:
Cooper J, Fazio RH. 1984. A new look at dissonance theory. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol.17:229–66
591: 416: 325: 468: 451: 1371: 1346: 1163: 1138: 764: 737: 353: 321: 1394:
Gentner, D.; Markman, A. B. (1997). "Structural alignment in analogy and similarity".
985: 1455: 1442: 882: 724:
Dillard, J (1994). "Rethinking the study of fear appeals: An emotional perspective".
555: 166: 1287: 1244: 1185:
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, IL: Row, Peterson.
1048:
Steele CM. 1988. The psychology of selfaffirmation.Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol.21:261–302
812: 485: 94:
Compliance was demonstrated through a series of laboratory experiments known as the
839: 701: 421: 345: 265: 250: 99: 51:
may be possible, though affective and cognitive components tend to be intertwined.
1362: 332:
Research has suggested multiple routes that cognitive dissonance can be reduced.
1228: 909: 176: 1106: 804: 693: 547: 499:
McGuire, W.; Lindzey, G.; Aronson, E. (1985). "Attitudes and attitude change".
1434: 1407: 1071: 398: 151: 79: 35: 1154: 874: 1347:"How judgments change following comparison of current and prior information" 275: 258: 43: 1380: 1322: 1279: 1236: 1172: 847: 599: 477: 71:. These three processes represent the different levels of attitude change. 1114: 1079: 165:
Important factors that influence the impact of emotional appeals include
27: 826:
Wood, Wendy (2000). "Attitude Change: Persuasion and Social Influence".
994: 147: 1314: 23: 1345:
Albarracin, D.; Wallace, H. M.; Hart, W.; Brown, R. D. (2012).
680:
Bandura, A. (1982). "Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency".
356:
occurs in which the source is forgotten after a period of time.
1258:
Crano, W. D.; Prislin, R. (2006). "Attitudes and persuasion".
611: 609: 501:
Handbook of Social Psychology: Special Fields and Applications
1301:
Anderson, N. H. (1959). "A test model of opinion change".
450:
Albarracin, Dolores; Shavitt, Sharon (4 January 2018).
320:
Cognitive dissonance, a theory originally developed by
324:(1957), is the idea that people experience a sense of 16:
Theory of change of associated beliefs and behaviours
304:
heuristic-systematic model of information processing
205:
Heuristic-systematic model of information processing
199:
Heuristic-systematic model of information processing
189:
heuristic-systematic model of information processing
1127:Rogers, Everett M:A history of communication study 967:"The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion" 786: 784: 1044: 1042: 965:Petty, Richard E.; Cacioppo, John T. (1986). 529: 527: 525: 523: 8: 793:Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 59:There are three bases for attitude change: 1143:Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 974:Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 246:the copier?" produced a 95% success rate. 1370: 1303:Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 1162: 993: 945:(5th ed.). New York: Harper Collins. 467: 1195:433-454. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum 78: 442: 38:to maintain cognitive consistency when 1272:10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190034 1137:Albarracin, D.; McNatt, P. S. (2005). 592:10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142015 249:Heuristic processing examples include 922:Langer, Blank, & Chanowitz (1978) 7: 1351:Basic and Applied Social Psychology 469:10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011911 765:10.1111/j.1745-3984.1975.tb01010.x 753:Journal of Educational Measurement 738:10.1111/j.1468-2885.1994.tb00094.x 661:Janis, Kaye, & Kirschner, 1965 14: 34:, as well as by the individual's 401: 183:Dual models: depth of processing 943:Influence: Science and practice 452:"Attitudes and Attitude Change" 840:10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.539 536:Journal of Conflict Resolution 1: 986:10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60214-2 432:Yale attitude change approach 1363:10.1080/01973533.2011.637480 298:Elaboration likelihood model 292:Elaboration likelihood model 193:elaboration likelihood model 1260:Annual Review of Psychology 1229:10.1037/0033-295x.110.3.472 910:10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1317 456:Annual Review of Psychology 310:Cognitive dissonance theory 1498: 1107:10.1037/0022-3514.72.1.184 805:10.1037/0022-3514.39.5.752 694:10.1037/0003-066x.37.2.122 652:Shelton & Rogers, 1981 548:10.1177/002200275800200106 313: 295: 202: 1408:10.1037/0003-066x.52.1.45 1072:10.1037/0022-3514.72.1.54 643:Maddux & Rogers, 1980 1155:10.1177/0146167204272180 875:10.1177/0146167296223004 863:Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull 1435:10.1023/a:1011113002473 941:Cialdini, R.B. (2008). 136:expectancy-value theory 1472:Psychological attitude 427:Reactance (psychology) 378:Comparative processing 365:Belief rationalization 150:plays a major role in 84: 1396:American Psychologist 1095:J. Pers. Soc. Psychol 1060:J. Pers. Soc. Psychol 898:J. Pers. Soc. Psychol 682:American Psychologist 220:Systematic processing 173:Affective forecasting 98:. Experiments led by 82: 1217:Psychological Review 726:Communication Theory 316:Cognitive dissonance 237:Heuristic processing 40:cognitive dissonance 360:persuasion attempt. 210:shortcuts known as 160:tobacco advertising 828:Annu. Rev. Psychol 580:Annu. Rev. Psychol 281:Milgram experiment 104:Swarthmore College 85: 1477:Children's rights 1423:Marketing Letters 409:Psychology portal 32:social influences 1489: 1447: 1446: 1418: 1412: 1411: 1391: 1385: 1384: 1374: 1342: 1336: 1333: 1327: 1326: 1315:10.1037/h0042539 1298: 1292: 1291: 1255: 1249: 1248: 1212: 1206: 1202: 1196: 1192: 1186: 1183: 1177: 1176: 1166: 1134: 1128: 1125: 1119: 1118: 1090: 1084: 1083: 1055: 1049: 1046: 1037: 1034: 1028: 1025: 1019: 1016: 1010: 1009: 997: 971: 962: 956: 953: 947: 946: 938: 932: 929: 923: 920: 914: 913: 893: 887: 886: 858: 852: 851: 823: 817: 816: 788: 779: 775: 769: 768: 748: 742: 741: 721: 715: 712: 706: 705: 677: 671: 668: 662: 659: 653: 650: 644: 641: 635: 632: 626: 623: 617: 613: 604: 603: 575: 569: 566: 560: 559: 531: 518: 515: 509: 508: 496: 490: 489: 471: 447: 411: 406: 405: 404: 341:Sorts of studies 334:Self-affirmation 156:social influence 96:Asch experiments 1497: 1496: 1492: 1491: 1490: 1488: 1487: 1486: 1462:Attitude change 1452: 1451: 1450: 1420: 1419: 1415: 1393: 1392: 1388: 1344: 1343: 1339: 1334: 1330: 1300: 1299: 1295: 1257: 1256: 1252: 1214: 1213: 1209: 1203: 1199: 1193: 1189: 1184: 1180: 1136: 1135: 1131: 1126: 1122: 1092: 1091: 1087: 1057: 1056: 1052: 1047: 1040: 1035: 1031: 1026: 1022: 1017: 1013: 1006: 969: 964: 963: 959: 954: 950: 940: 939: 935: 930: 926: 921: 917: 895: 894: 890: 860: 859: 855: 825: 824: 820: 790: 789: 782: 776: 772: 750: 749: 745: 723: 722: 718: 713: 709: 679: 678: 674: 669: 665: 660: 656: 651: 647: 642: 638: 634:Leventhal, 1970 633: 629: 624: 620: 614: 607: 577: 576: 572: 567: 563: 533: 532: 521: 516: 512: 498: 497: 493: 449: 448: 444: 440: 417:Attitudinal fix 407: 402: 400: 397: 380: 367: 343: 318: 312: 300: 294: 239: 222: 207: 201: 185: 145: 128:Internalization 125: 123:Internalization 113: 77: 69:internalization 57: 49:attitude change 22:are associated 17: 12: 11: 5: 1495: 1493: 1485: 1484: 1479: 1474: 1469: 1467:Human behavior 1464: 1454: 1453: 1449: 1448: 1429:(2): 131–144. 1413: 1386: 1337: 1328: 1293: 1250: 1207: 1197: 1187: 1178: 1149:(6): 719–733. 1129: 1120: 1085: 1050: 1038: 1029: 1020: 1011: 1004: 957: 948: 933: 924: 915: 904:(5): 1317–28. 888: 853: 818: 799:(5): 752–766. 780: 770: 743: 732:(4): 295–323. 716: 707: 688:(2): 122–147. 672: 663: 654: 645: 636: 627: 618: 605: 570: 561: 519: 510: 491: 462:(1): 299–327. 441: 439: 436: 435: 434: 429: 424: 419: 413: 412: 396: 393: 379: 376: 366: 363: 362: 361: 357: 354:sleeper effect 342: 339: 314:Main article: 311: 308: 296:Main article: 293: 290: 289: 288: 284: 279:change is the 273: 270: 261:, and liking. 238: 235: 221: 218: 203:Main article: 200: 197: 184: 181: 144: 141: 124: 121: 116:Identification 112: 111:Identification 109: 76: 73: 65:identification 56: 53: 15: 13: 10: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 1494: 1483: 1480: 1478: 1475: 1473: 1470: 1468: 1465: 1463: 1460: 1459: 1457: 1444: 1440: 1436: 1432: 1428: 1424: 1417: 1414: 1409: 1405: 1401: 1397: 1390: 1387: 1382: 1378: 1373: 1368: 1364: 1360: 1356: 1352: 1348: 1341: 1338: 1332: 1329: 1324: 1320: 1316: 1312: 1308: 1304: 1297: 1294: 1289: 1285: 1281: 1277: 1273: 1269: 1265: 1261: 1254: 1251: 1246: 1242: 1238: 1234: 1230: 1226: 1223:(3): 472–89. 1222: 1218: 1211: 1208: 1201: 1198: 1191: 1188: 1182: 1179: 1174: 1170: 1165: 1160: 1156: 1152: 1148: 1144: 1140: 1133: 1130: 1124: 1121: 1116: 1112: 1108: 1104: 1101:(1): 184–95. 1100: 1096: 1089: 1086: 1081: 1077: 1073: 1069: 1065: 1061: 1054: 1051: 1045: 1043: 1039: 1033: 1030: 1024: 1021: 1015: 1012: 1007: 1005:9780120152193 1001: 996: 991: 987: 983: 979: 975: 968: 961: 958: 952: 949: 944: 937: 934: 931:Sherif (1936) 928: 925: 919: 916: 911: 907: 903: 899: 892: 889: 884: 880: 876: 872: 869:(3): 257–68. 868: 864: 857: 854: 849: 845: 841: 837: 833: 829: 822: 819: 814: 810: 806: 802: 798: 794: 787: 785: 781: 774: 771: 766: 762: 758: 754: 747: 744: 739: 735: 731: 727: 720: 717: 711: 708: 703: 699: 695: 691: 687: 683: 676: 673: 667: 664: 658: 655: 649: 646: 640: 637: 631: 628: 622: 619: 612: 610: 606: 601: 597: 593: 589: 585: 581: 574: 571: 565: 562: 557: 553: 549: 545: 541: 537: 530: 528: 526: 524: 520: 514: 511: 506: 502: 495: 492: 487: 483: 479: 475: 470: 465: 461: 457: 453: 446: 443: 437: 433: 430: 428: 425: 423: 420: 418: 415: 414: 410: 399: 394: 392: 388: 384: 377: 375: 371: 364: 358: 355: 351: 350: 349: 347: 340: 338: 335: 330: 327: 323: 317: 309: 307: 305: 299: 291: 285: 282: 277: 274: 271: 267: 264: 263: 262: 260: 256: 252: 247: 243: 236: 234: 230: 226: 219: 217: 215: 214: 206: 198: 196: 194: 190: 182: 180: 178: 174: 170: 168: 167:self-efficacy 163: 161: 157: 153: 149: 143:Emotion-based 142: 140: 137: 132: 129: 122: 120: 117: 110: 108: 105: 101: 97: 92: 89: 81: 74: 72: 70: 66: 62: 54: 52: 50: 45: 41: 37: 33: 29: 25: 21: 1426: 1422: 1416: 1399: 1395: 1389: 1357:(1): 44–55. 1354: 1350: 1340: 1331: 1306: 1302: 1296: 1263: 1259: 1253: 1220: 1216: 1210: 1200: 1190: 1181: 1146: 1142: 1132: 1123: 1098: 1094: 1088: 1066:(1): 54–65. 1063: 1059: 1053: 1032: 1023: 1014: 977: 973: 960: 951: 942: 936: 927: 918: 901: 897: 891: 866: 862: 856: 831: 827: 821: 796: 792: 773: 759:(2): 67–85. 756: 752: 746: 729: 725: 719: 710: 685: 681: 675: 666: 657: 648: 639: 630: 621: 583: 579: 573: 564: 542:(1): 51–60. 539: 535: 513: 504: 500: 494: 459: 455: 445: 422:Fear appeals 389: 385: 381: 372: 368: 346:Carl Hovland 344: 331: 319: 301: 266:Social proof 251:social proof 248: 244: 240: 231: 227: 223: 211: 208: 186: 171: 164: 146: 133: 126: 114: 100:Solomon Asch 93: 86: 58: 48: 18: 1309:: 371–381. 1266:: 345–374. 995:10983/26083 980:: 123–205. 834:: 539–570. 586:: 591–621. 255:reciprocity 177:methodology 1456:Categories 507:: 233–346. 438:References 213:heuristics 152:persuasion 88:Compliance 75:Compliance 61:compliance 36:motivation 1482:Parenting 1443:140902413 1402:: 45–56. 883:146624512 778:Guilford. 556:145642577 322:Festinger 276:Authority 259:authority 44:affective 28:behaviors 20:Attitudes 1381:23599557 1323:13793435 1288:14332375 1280:16318599 1245:15881635 1237:12885111 1205:Erlbaum. 1173:15833901 848:10751980 813:39212150 616:Erlbaum. 600:14744228 486:41104994 478:28841390 395:See also 191:and the 1372:3627214 1164:4803285 1115:9008380 1080:9008374 702:3377361 148:Emotion 24:beliefs 1441:  1379:  1369:  1321:  1286:  1278:  1243:  1235:  1171:  1161:  1113:  1078:  1002:  881:  846:  811:  700:  598:  554:  484:  476:  67:, and 1439:S2CID 1284:S2CID 1241:S2CID 970:(PDF) 879:S2CID 809:S2CID 698:S2CID 552:S2CID 482:S2CID 326:guilt 55:Bases 1377:PMID 1319:PMID 1276:PMID 1233:PMID 1169:PMID 1111:PMID 1076:PMID 1000:ISBN 844:PMID 596:PMID 474:PMID 134:The 26:and 1431:doi 1404:doi 1367:PMC 1359:doi 1311:doi 1268:doi 1225:doi 1221:110 1159:PMC 1151:doi 1103:doi 1068:doi 990:hdl 982:doi 906:doi 871:doi 836:doi 801:doi 761:doi 734:doi 690:doi 588:doi 544:doi 464:doi 102:of 1458:: 1437:. 1427:12 1425:. 1400:52 1398:. 1375:. 1365:. 1355:34 1353:. 1349:. 1317:. 1307:59 1305:. 1282:. 1274:. 1264:57 1262:. 1239:. 1231:. 1219:. 1167:. 1157:. 1147:31 1145:. 1141:. 1109:. 1099:72 1097:. 1074:. 1064:72 1062:. 1041:^ 998:. 988:. 978:19 976:. 972:. 902:74 900:. 877:. 867:22 865:. 842:. 832:51 830:. 807:. 797:39 795:. 783:^ 757:12 755:. 728:. 696:. 686:37 684:. 608:^ 594:. 584:55 582:. 550:. 538:. 522:^ 503:. 480:. 472:. 460:69 458:. 454:. 257:, 253:, 195:. 154:, 63:, 1445:. 1433:: 1410:. 1406:: 1383:. 1361:: 1325:. 1313:: 1290:. 1270:: 1247:. 1227:: 1175:. 1153:: 1117:. 1105:: 1082:. 1070:: 1008:. 992:: 984:: 912:. 908:: 885:. 873:: 850:. 838:: 815:. 803:: 767:. 763:: 740:. 736:: 730:4 704:. 692:: 602:. 590:: 558:. 546:: 540:2 505:2 488:. 466::

Index

Attitudes
beliefs
behaviors
social influences
motivation
cognitive dissonance
affective
compliance
identification
internalization

Compliance
Asch experiments
Solomon Asch
Swarthmore College
Identification
Internalization
expectancy-value theory
Emotion
persuasion
social influence
tobacco advertising
self-efficacy
Affective forecasting
methodology
heuristic-systematic model of information processing
elaboration likelihood model
Heuristic-systematic model of information processing
heuristics
social proof

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.

↑