Knowledge (XXG)

Caridoid escape reaction

Source 📝

325:
beta pathway, the signal can then pass a chemical synapse, the information is sent to a sensory interneuron (SI) of either type A (which fires phasically in response to input) or type C, (which fire tonically). From there, the signal is sent across an electrical synapse to the LGI. The LGI can also receive the sensory input directly through an electrical synapse by way of the alpha pathway. This bypasses the SIs. The LGI can pass the signal to the MoG's by two paths one of which passes the signal to the Motor Giant through direct electrical synapses. The MoG then passes the signal to the FF muscles. It can also use a second route across a fast electrical synapse to a premotor interneuron called the segmental giant (SG), two of which are located in each segment. The SGs then relays the signal to fast flexor motor neurons. The end result is a powerful and rapid flexion.
309:(also called septate synapses). As a result, the LGI functions as one giant, continuous neuron, such as the MG. The LGI's large diameter and the efficiency and speed of electrical synapses, make the LGI-mediated escape especially rapid and effective. The LGI only innervates the first three rostral segments of the tail and is activated within 10 ms from when a mechanical stimulus is presented to the abdomen. When flexion occurs, only the first three segments flex and the tail fan is not directed under the body and forward but rather straight down. The unflexed segments increase the length of the paddle. The result is the rear end of the animal is directed upward and forward causing the animal to tumble or somersault forward. The tail is then rapidly extended, and this is usually followed by directed swimming. 275:. These fibers terminate in the last abdominal segment. The response is triggered by abrupt tactile stimuli to the head or alarming visual stimuli. This results in the firing of all motor giant (MoG) neurons and the flexion of all the phasic fast flexor (FF) muscles in the abdomen, which rapidly curls the tail fan and last three segments underneath the crayfish. This produces a thrust that directs water towards the offending stimulus while propelling the crayfish directly backward. This behavior is then followed by the extension of the tail, which prepares it for swimming or the execution of another tail-flip. The extension and swimming phases will be discussed in the next escape variant. 391:
the inhibitory effect is lost and the LGI in severed abdomen generates strong spikes since its threshold had decreased substantially. This is an indication that the behavior is mediated in the brain or the thorax of the crayfish. Most non-giant and MGI tail flips are also suppressed by restraint, so only a few non-giant systems are active to allow the crayfish to carefully wriggle free. The setup of this system seems to be designed to prevent the unnecessary use of all or nothing tail flips or ineffective tail flips when the situation requires more careful maneuvering.
148:. The paper stated that command neurons were neurons (or small sets of neurons) carrying the entire command signal for a natural behavioral response. According to the authors, command neurons were both necessary and sufficient in the production of a behavioral response. The concept of command neuron-mediated behaviors was both ground breaking and controversial, since determining command neuron-mediated behaviors was a problematic process due to difficulties in understanding and interpreting anatomical and behavioral data. 201:
pairs of walking legs. This is the crayfish's primary mode of locomotion. The abdominal section of the crayfish is divided into seven segments. These segments are flexibly interconnected, forming the tail. Normally, the tail is held in an extended position to aid in maneuvering and balancing. The first five segments are similar and the two terminal segments are modified into a tail fan, a region with high surface area that acts as the blade of a paddle in the escape response. This region contains the
288:
interneurons, most likely because they do not produce depolarizations in the sensory neurons that are above the thresholds required to initiate these behaviors. Instead they involve one or a few of the smaller neurons innervating the tail. The latency periods for these escape mechanisms are longer, ranging from 80 to 500 ms. However, this slower swimming behavior allows for flexibility, since the crayfish can use visual stimuli and steer itself in a selected direction.
98: 1586: 418:
decreased. Aggressive dominant males have a moderate reduction in tail flips while the subordinates have a much lower occurrence of tail flips. This presents a paradox since this means that subordinates are more likely to get killed. However, it was found that subordinates are more likely to use non-giant-mediated escape, indicating that the reduction in tail flips and enhancement of non giant escape is adaptive.
333:
prevent these events. The extensor muscles are inhibited to prevent competition between the extensor and flexors. Rapid IPSPs presented at the muscle receptor organ (MRO) prevent the stretch receptor from initiating extension while they are also presented at the fast extensor motor neurons and the fast extensor muscles. The circuits responsible for slow flexion and extension are also inhibited.
20: 1598: 360:
after the completion of the tail flip. The non-giant swimming occurs independently of the LGI response since direct stimulation of the LGI with the electrodes results in a tail flip but not the subsequent non-giant swimming. This swimming response seems to be a fixed action pattern mediated by a central pattern generator since it does not require
422:
to be mediated by different cell receptors. Differences in the effects of serotonin on the behavior of the crayfish seem to be the result of differences in the populations of these receptors. It is unknown how these modulation processes convey the information to the LG, and how the behavioral changes are precipitated.
296:
by non-giant circuitry have lower amplitudes and longer durations while the recordings in the FF muscles are more erratic and have smaller amplitudes since they receive smaller EPSPs from the MoGs. So while it is hard to observe differences in MG and non-escapes, this property can be used to distinguish the two.
184:
flexion is a hundredth of a second (10 milliseconds). Finally, the caridoid escape reflex requires that neurons be able to complete the arduous task of synchronizing the flexion of several abdominal segments. The speed, coordination, and decisiveness of the process seem to be the main attributes to its success.
382:
LGI itself is modulated by the behavior. Feeding increases the threshold for the tail flip, and even when the tail is tapped, no spikes are initiated. As a result, it takes an especially strong stimulus to elicit the tail flip. This prevents the crayfish from going hungry unless it is absolutely necessary.
408:
Repeated tapping of the abdomen leads to habituation of the tail flip mechanism. However, self–habituation is prevented by command neuron–derived inhibition because when a tail flip is begun, the mechanisms that induce habituation are repressed. The habituation occurs at the level of the A type and C
346:
of the MRO are located on the dorsal side of the abdomen, each spanning the joint of two segments. They are innervated by proprioreceptors that detect the stretch in extensor muscles when the flexors are contracted. They then fire phasically or tonically. It then excites the motor neurons in the fast
295:
Non-giant escape circuitry was found to be activated more during frontal attacks, but was rarely involved in the initial escape during a rear attack. Non-giant escape is often used after an initial giant interneuron-mediated tail flip. Compared to MG escape, the potentials produced in the MoG neurons
390:
When a crayfish is held by its carapace either in the water or in the air, it does not respond with a tail flip when it receives sensory stimuli that would normally elicit the response. This inhibitory behavior does not seem to be mediated by the abdomen. When the thorax-abdomen junction is severed,
332:
A number of processes are inhibited once the initial flexion has begun. Further LGI spiking, influx of sensory information from the tail, MoG spiking and FF contraction are all inhibited so that a flexion and extension cycle can be completed before another flexion begins. Long IPSPs are generated to
312:
The LGI is one of the few examples of a true command neuron. It passes the necessity and sufficiency principle put forward by Kupfermann and Weiss. Wiersma's initial experiments showed that direct stimulation of the LGI was a sufficient release for the tail flip. In 1981, it was Olson and Krasne who
245:
known as the lateral giant neurons and the medial giant neurons. These interneurons play important roles in escape swimming. Their large diameter allows for rapid conduction since there is less current leakage. Their projections extend through the third root in each ganglion, and Furshpan and Potter
421:
Changes in social status should correlate with changes in serotonin levels, resulting in changes in the escape strategies used by the crayfish. However this is only true when submissive crayfish become dominant, and not the reverse. The neuromodulatory processes of facilitation and inhibition seem
394:
The tail flip normally induces inhibition in an absolute fashion, such that it takes precedence over all the other tasks the segments are performing. However, during these situations, the effectiveness of the tail flip is reduced, so the inhibitory processes need to occur in a relative manner and a
291:
Non-giant escape often occurs during situations where lateral or medial giant-mediated escape may not be beneficial or during times where those behaviors are suppressed. This allows the crayfish to attack offenders, escape during feeding, or wriggle free when it has been restrained by the carapace.
200:
that reflects muscular and neural segmentation. The anterior portion of the crayfish is the cephalothorax region. The region rostral to the cephalic groove, which separates the head and thorax region, is characterized by the presence of eyes, antennae and claws while the region caudal contains four
381:
Crayfish often find themselves in a conflicting situation where they are performing the highly motivated behavior of feeding when they suddenly receive a tail flip stimulus. Often, the crayfish will not perform a tail flip in this situation. This is because when a crayfish is actively feeding, the
324:
The LGI receives its sensory input from the hair-like projections found on the edge of the tail fan. The sensory information is sent from bipolar receptors connected to the hair that are directionally sensitive. The path of the signal varies from here depending on the strength of the input. In the
287:
Less abrupt or gradually intensifying stimuli evoke locomotion behavior similar to that seen in giant interneuron-mediated behaviors. However, the movements are less stereotyped and do not appear to occur in an all or nothing manner. They are so named because they lack the involvement of the giant
183:
are used like a paddle to propel the crustacean away from harm using powerful abdominal flexions. The entire process occurs in a fraction of a second as movements are generated within two hundredths of a second (20 milliseconds) from the original trigger stimulus and the period of latency after a
341:
When the LGI-mediated inhibition subsides, the re-extension process begins. This process is mediated by the MRO and tail fan hair receptors, which were inhibited during the flexion portion of the escape behavior. The MRO is responsible for the initiation of extension and the inhibition of flexion
328:
The convergence many inputs on the LGI and divergence of few signals show how the LGI functions as a decision making coincidence detector. It takes the information from both the alpha and the beta pathways and if the timing of the spikes is synchronous, the behavior is produced. If the timing is
178:
depends on the region of the crayfish that is stimulated but all forms require abdominal contractions. When a strong, unpleasant tactile stimulus is presented, such as a burst of water or the prod of a probe, a stereotyped behavior occurs in which the muscular tail and wide tail fan region of the
1247:
provides an idea of the organization of the segmental ganglia in the tail of the crayfish. The second diagram on the page is a transverse section through the tail that highlights the positions of the LGI, MGI and non-giant neurons. At the bottom of the page it also has diagrams of the tail flips
443:
that diverged from the crayfish lineage very early on. It is likely that this mechanism was replaced by the tail flip since the wide surface area of the tail made this behavior more selectively advantageous. This most likely occurred when the ancestral flexor motor neurons in each segment formed
359:
Non-giant-mediated responses are initiated after the tail flip, creating cycles of flexion followed by extension. This non-giant system is activated parallel to the LGI circuit when the hair cells receive input. However, this behavior has a longer delay that allows the onset of swimming to occur
430:
It has been hypothesized that the tail flip is derived from an ancient limb protraction driven (as opposed to tail flexion-driven) mechanism. This is because the SGs appear to be modified limb motor neurons whose peripheral axons affect the legs and swimmerets, but end blindly without any known
417:
The feedback from social situations affect a crayfish's ability to perform a tail flip. Serotonin levels are affected by social status. High levels are associated with aggressive behavior and a reduction in the frequency of tail flips performed. This is because the excitability of the LGI is
250:
account for the speed of the escape mechanism and display some features of chemical synapses such as LTP and LTD. Variations in escape response characteristic depend on the location where the crayfish body is prodded or attacked and also depend on which of the giant neurons is stimulated.
278:
Unfortunately, this variant of the tail-flip escape has not been extensively studied. Further studies should focus on this escape variant, paying special attention to exactly how visual information is processed and then converted into neuronal signals that produce a tail flip response.
133:(MG or MGI) would result in the execution of a tail flip. Wiersma and K. Ikeda then proposed the term "command neuron" in their 1964 publication, and applied it to the giant interneuron's ability to command the expression of the escape response. This was the first description of a 329:
asynchronous, the later input is blocked by reducing the driving force of the signal and increasing the threshold voltage. The early synapse also causes current to leak through the active synapse, resulting in weak EPSPs that are ultimately unable to generate the tail flip.
232:
that innervate the extensor muscles, while the third root contains only motor neuron projections that extend into the flexor muscles. The last segment contains the fusion of two ganglia. The ganglia here also receive sensory input from the sensitive hairs on the tail fan.
84:
that make the crustacean quickly swim backwards through the water and away from danger. The type of response depends on the part of the crustacean stimulated, but this behavior is complex and is regulated both spatially and temporally through the interactions of several
313:
showed that the LGI fulfilled the condition of necessity, because when spikes were inhibited in the LGI through introducing a hyperpolarizing current, no motor activity was produced in response to a stimulus that would have originally elicited the tail flip.
399:
of the LGI. It is most likely that during restraint or feeding, the inhibitory process is mediated by inhibitory synaptic input on distal dendrites of the LGI . As a result, these signals would have to compete with other inputs for the control of the LGI.
434:
It has been speculated that the ancestral escape mechanism was most likely a backwards jump due to the simultaneous protraction of the legs driven by the ancestors of the Giant Fibers. This behavior was probably similar to the escape system found in a
372:
The production of a tail flip is not always beneficial. Evolution has allowed the crayfish to be more flexible by presenting several control systems that will prevent the tail flip in situations where it will most likely be unnecessary or ineffective.
448:
mechanism seems to be similar to this ancestral state because a large diameter axon in the dorsal nerve chord facilitates limb promoter motor neurons. This seems to match the ancestral condition, but it is not known for sure whether the circuitry is
223:
run down the entire length of the abdomen and communicate with one another through projections. The first five abdominal segments each have their own ganglion, that contains three roots with outward projections. The first has mixed sensory and
304:
The lateral giant (LG)-mediated escape mechanism is the most extensively analyzed form of the tail flip. The LG is not actually one neuron, but rather a group of closely associated neurons arranged end to end and connected by
124:
present in the tail were responsible for the reaction. The aforementioned neuronal fibres consist of a pair of lateral giant interneurons and a pair of medial giant interneurons, and Wiersma found that stimulating just one
236:
Each ganglion contains the body of one motor giant neuron (MoG), powerful and large bodied motor neurons whose projections innervate the five fast flexor (FF) muscles found in a segment and interact with them through
1248:
caused by stimulation of the LGI (on the left half of the diagram) and the MGI (on the right half of the diagram). This accurate diagram appears to be similar to that found in Wine and Krasne's 1972 publication.
1209:
Roberts, A., Krasne, F. B., Hagiwara, G., Wine, J. J., and Krarner, A. P. (1982) "Segmental giant: Evidence for a driver neuron interposed between command and motor neurons in the crayfish escape system".
409:
type interneurons, which experience synaptic depression. The habituation process is also mediated further up the circuit through the buildup of tonic inhibition, brought on by the repeated stimulation.
431:
function. It is known that another effect of Giant Fiber excitation is limb promotion which suggests that the premotor limb interneurons may be ancestors of the Giant Fibers.
170:
it was discovered that the tail-flip mechanism is characterized by a decisive, all-or-nothing quality that inhibits all unnecessary behaviors while generating a
641: 1103:
Vu, E. T.; Lee, S. C.; Krasne, F. B. (1993), "The mechanism of tonic inhibition of crayfish escape behavior: distal inhibition and its functional",
1150: 1290: 1221:
Wine, J.J. and Hagiwara, G. (1977) "Crayfish Escape Behavior I. The Structure of Efferent and Afferent Neurons Involved in Abdominal Extension".
1241:
includes information on the biology and conservation of lobsters. The majority can be applied to crayfish due to common ancestry and homology.
785: 1318: 941: 863: 632: 535: 1335: 395:
tail flip should occur only if absolutely necessary. During absolute inhibition is directed by more proximal synapses controlling the
246:
found that the synapses they subsequently made with the MoG passed depolarizing currents in a direct and unidirectional manner. These
351:
detect the resulting movement caused by the tail flip when activated, they would fire and excite the fast extensor motor neurons.
705:
Wiersma, C. A.; Ikeda, K. (1964), "Interneurons Commanding Swimmeret Movements in the Crayfish, Procambarus Clarki (girard)",
1534: 144:
This concept was further fleshed out with more specific and stringent conditions in 1972 when Kupfermann and Weiss published
1251: 993:
Kramer, A. P.; Krasne, F. B. (1984), "Crayfish escape behavior: production of tailflips without giant fiber activity",
1283: 959:
Reichert, H.; Wine, J. J. (1983), "Coordination of lateral giant and non-giant systems in crayfish escape behavior",
638: 1539: 1524: 1340: 530: 510: 126: 347:
extensor muscles while directly exciting an inhibitory neuron that prevents contractions in the FF muscles. The
1602: 515: 130: 1629: 1397: 76:
The reaction, most extensively researched in crayfish, allows crustaceans to escape predators through rapid
396: 1624: 1590: 1554: 1313: 1276: 1370: 1147: 272: 1238: 1032:"Escape behavior and escape circuit activation in juvenile crayfish during prey-predator interactions" 111: 1447: 1380: 1360: 1355: 525: 171: 48: 1514: 1452: 1437: 1402: 824: 540: 162: 116: 103: 1477: 1407: 1375: 1350: 976: 816: 756: 450: 306: 247: 666:"Giant Nerve Fiber System of the Crayfish. A Contribution to Comparative Physiology of Synapse" 1498: 1263: 1199: 1130: 1053: 1010: 937: 911: 859: 808: 722: 685: 628: 1564: 1559: 1472: 1191: 1120: 1112: 1086: 1043: 1002: 968: 903: 800: 748: 714: 677: 361: 316:
The LGI contains the following three phases, occurring over the span of about 100 ms:
238: 44: 1529: 1432: 1427: 1154: 645: 175: 40: 19: 1031: 665: 97: 1549: 1422: 1417: 1365: 1125: 1116: 932:
Neuroethology : an introduction to the neurophysiological fundamentals of behavior
930: 520: 160:
have researched this behavior extensively for over fifty years in the crayfish species
138: 134: 1071: 804: 786:"Fifty years of a command neuron: the neurobiology of escape behavior in the crayfish" 1618: 1412: 1345: 1299: 1176: 760: 718: 436: 157: 980: 1569: 1519: 1457: 888: 820: 229: 264:
The medial giants innervate the entire ventral nerve chord. Their cell bodies and
1493: 1462: 1442: 225: 197: 121: 1244: 1467: 752: 54: 1006: 681: 625:
Behavioral neurobiology : the cellular organization of natural behavior
348: 77: 1057: 915: 812: 726: 689: 268:
begin in the brain and collect sensory information presented by visual and
1203: 1134: 1090: 1014: 141:
of a neuron could precipitate complex innate behaviors in some organisms.
1330: 1255: 1195: 505: 265: 213: 193: 70: 51: 565:
Krasne, F. B.; Wine, J. J. (1987), "Evasion responses of the crayfish",
972: 907: 488: 343: 220: 58: 1048: 1324: 478: 465: 216: 209: 202: 180: 86: 81: 66: 205:. The abdominal segments contain swimmerets, which aid in swimming. 739:
Kupfermann, I.; Weiss, K.R. (1978), "The command neuron concept",
269: 101:
The tail-flip escape behavior was first described in the crayfish
96: 62: 18: 1268: 242: 1272: 1030:
Herberholz, Jens; Sen, Marjorie M.; Edwards, Donald H. (2004),
1177:"Command Interneurons in the Crayfish Central Nervous System" 471: 208:
The anterior five segments of the crayfish house the massive
486:, which is the root of the name of the taxonomic infraorder 228:
innervating swimmerets while the second has sensory and
1072:"The Structural Basis of an Innate Behavioural Pattern" 93:
Discovery of the first command neuron-mediated behavior
889:"The Organization of Escape Behaviour in the Crayfish" 784:
Edwards, D. H.; Heitler, W. J.; Krasne, F. B. (1999),
444:
connections with one of these limb motor neurons. The
196:
crustaceans, the crayfish possesses a hard, segmented
114:
first described the tail-flip escape in the crayfish
627:, Sunderland, Mass.: Sinauer Associates Publishers, 1507: 1486: 1390: 1306: 929: 1025: 1023: 858:, Sunderland, Massachusetts: Sinauer Associates, 492:, a large group of crustaceans also known as the 954: 952: 1264:Cornell University's Neuroethology Course page 1284: 779: 777: 775: 773: 771: 769: 700: 698: 659: 657: 655: 653: 241:. The ganglia also contain two sets of giant 137:-mediated behavior and it indicated that the 8: 618: 616: 614: 612: 610: 608: 606: 604: 602: 600: 598: 596: 560: 558: 556: 849: 847: 845: 843: 594: 592: 590: 588: 586: 584: 582: 580: 578: 576: 426:Evolution of the tail flip escape mechanism 156:Behavioral neurobiologists in the field of 1291: 1277: 1269: 882: 880: 878: 876: 874: 1175:Atwood, H. L.; Wiersma, C. A. G. (1967), 1124: 1047: 368:Modulation and plasticity of the response 1223:The Journal of Comparative Physiology-A 552: 364:for physical and temporal maintenance. 639:Chapter 7: Escape Behavior in Crayfish 23:Animated representation of lobstering. 16:Innate escape mechanism by crustaceans 536:Coincidence detection in neurobiology 342:through a reflex like mechanism. The 255:Response variants and their circuitry 7: 1597: 961:Journal of Comparative Physiology A 887:Wine, J. J.; Krasne, F. B. (1972), 1117:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-10-04379.1993 14: 567:Aims and Methods in Neuroethology 174:for escape swimming. The type of 1596: 1585: 1584: 1341:Central pattern generator (CPG) 1184:Journal of Experimental Biology 1079:Journal of Experimental Biology 1036:Journal of Experimental Biology 896:Journal of Experimental Biology 80:flexions that produce powerful 1535:Frog hearing and communication 1: 805:10.1016/S0166-2236(98)01340-X 300:Lateral giant-mediated escape 719:10.1016/0010-406X(64)90153-7 466: 260:Medial giant-mediated escape 936:, Berlin: Springer-Verlag, 1648: 1540:Infrared sensing in snakes 1525:Jamming avoidance response 1212:Journal of Neurophysiology 995:Journal of Neurophysiology 928:Ewert, Jörg-Peter (1980), 854:Camhi, Jeffrey M. (1984), 670:Journal of Neurophysiology 664:Wiersma, C. A. G. (1947), 472: 464:is derived from the Greek 146:The Command Neuron Concept 1580: 753:10.1017/S0140525X00059057 623:Carew, Thomas J. (2000), 531:Central pattern generator 511:Lateral giant interneuron 476:(pronounced "kah-RISS"), 127:lateral giant interneuron 120:and noted that the giant 1545:Caridoid escape reaction 1254:presents information on 1239:The Lobster Conservatory 1007:10.1152/jn.1984.52.2.189 516:Medial giant interneuron 131:medial giant interneuron 29:caridoid escape reaction 1398:Theodore Holmes Bullock 1245:Neural and tail anatomy 1105:Journal of Neuroscience 793:Trends in Neurosciences 682:10.1152/jn.1947.10.1.23 1555:Surface wave detection 397:spike-initiating zones 320:1 – Flexion production 166:. Based on studies of 152:Research with crayfish 107: 24: 1371:Anti-Hebbian learning 1091:10.1242/jeb.112.1.283 644:June 9, 2011, at the 100: 22: 1448:Bernhard Hassenstein 1381:Ultrasound avoidance 1356:Fixed action pattern 1319:Coincidence detector 1196:10.1242/jeb.46.2.249 1161:online Greek lexicon 1070:Wine, J. J. (1984), 707:Comp Biochem Physiol 526:Fixed action pattern 172:fixed action pattern 1515:Animal echolocation 1453:Werner E. Reichardt 1403:Walter Heiligenberg 541:Pain in crustaceans 307:electrical synapses 248:electrical synapses 163:Procambarus clarkii 129:(LG or LGI) or one 117:Procambarus clarkii 104:Procambarus clarkii 1478:Fernando Nottebohm 1376:Sound localization 1351:Lateral inhibition 1153:2018-09-04 at the 973:10.1007/BF00610337 908:10.1242/jeb.56.1.1 386:Carapace restraint 108: 25: 1612: 1611: 1499:Slice preparation 1361:Krogh's Principle 1336:Feature detection 1252:Lobster Body Plan 1169:Suggested reading 1111:(10): 4379–4393, 1049:10.1242/jeb.00992 1042:(11): 1855–1863, 943:978-0-387-09790-9 865:978-0-87893-075-3 634:978-0-87893-084-5 239:chemical synapses 1637: 1600: 1599: 1588: 1587: 1565:Mechanoreception 1560:Electroreception 1473:Masakazu Konishi 1438:Jörg-Peter Ewert 1293: 1286: 1279: 1270: 1206: 1181: 1162: 1144: 1138: 1137: 1128: 1100: 1094: 1093: 1076: 1067: 1061: 1060: 1051: 1027: 1018: 1017: 990: 984: 983: 956: 947: 946: 935: 925: 919: 918: 893: 884: 869: 868: 851: 838: 837: 836: 835: 829: 823:, archived from 790: 781: 764: 763: 736: 730: 729: 702: 693: 692: 661: 648: 637: 620: 571: 570: 562: 475: 474: 469: 362:sensory feedback 337:2 – Re-extension 283:Non-giant escape 219:. Six abdominal 188:Anatomy involved 112:C. A. G. Wiersma 31:, also known as 1647: 1646: 1640: 1639: 1638: 1636: 1635: 1634: 1615: 1614: 1613: 1608: 1576: 1530:Vision in toads 1503: 1482: 1433:Erich von Holst 1428:Karl von Frisch 1386: 1302: 1297: 1235: 1179: 1174: 1171: 1166: 1165: 1155:Wayback Machine 1145: 1141: 1102: 1101: 1097: 1074: 1069: 1068: 1064: 1029: 1028: 1021: 992: 991: 987: 958: 957: 950: 944: 927: 926: 922: 891: 886: 885: 872: 866: 853: 852: 841: 833: 831: 827: 788: 783: 782: 767: 741:Behav Brain Sci 738: 737: 733: 704: 703: 696: 663: 662: 651: 646:Wayback Machine 635: 622: 621: 574: 564: 563: 554: 549: 502: 494:caridean shrimp 459: 428: 415: 406: 388: 379: 370: 357: 339: 322: 302: 285: 262: 257: 190: 176:escape response 154: 95: 41:escape behavior 39:, is an innate 17: 12: 11: 5: 1645: 1644: 1641: 1633: 1632: 1630:Marine biology 1627: 1617: 1616: 1610: 1609: 1607: 1606: 1594: 1581: 1578: 1577: 1575: 1574: 1573: 1572: 1562: 1557: 1552: 1550:Vocal learning 1547: 1542: 1537: 1532: 1527: 1522: 1517: 1511: 1509: 1505: 1504: 1502: 1501: 1496: 1490: 1488: 1484: 1483: 1481: 1480: 1475: 1470: 1465: 1460: 1455: 1450: 1445: 1440: 1435: 1430: 1425: 1423:Donald Kennedy 1420: 1418:Donald Griffin 1415: 1410: 1408:Niko Tinbergen 1405: 1400: 1394: 1392: 1388: 1387: 1385: 1384: 1378: 1373: 1368: 1366:Hebbian theory 1363: 1358: 1353: 1348: 1343: 1338: 1333: 1328: 1321: 1316: 1310: 1308: 1304: 1303: 1298: 1296: 1295: 1288: 1281: 1273: 1267: 1266: 1261: 1260: 1259: 1249: 1234: 1233:External links 1231: 1230: 1229: 1219: 1207: 1190:(2): 249–261, 1170: 1167: 1164: 1163: 1139: 1095: 1085:(1): 283–319, 1062: 1019: 1001:(2): 189–211, 985: 948: 942: 920: 870: 864: 839: 799:(4): 153–161, 765: 731: 694: 649: 633: 572: 551: 550: 548: 545: 544: 543: 538: 533: 528: 523: 521:Command neuron 518: 513: 508: 501: 498: 458: 455: 427: 424: 414: 411: 405: 402: 387: 384: 378: 375: 369: 366: 356: 353: 338: 335: 321: 318: 301: 298: 284: 281: 261: 258: 256: 253: 189: 186: 153: 150: 139:depolarization 135:command neuron 94: 91: 15: 13: 10: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 1643: 1642: 1631: 1628: 1626: 1625:Neuroethology 1623: 1622: 1620: 1605: 1604: 1595: 1593: 1592: 1583: 1582: 1579: 1571: 1568: 1567: 1566: 1563: 1561: 1558: 1556: 1553: 1551: 1548: 1546: 1543: 1541: 1538: 1536: 1533: 1531: 1528: 1526: 1523: 1521: 1518: 1516: 1513: 1512: 1510: 1506: 1500: 1497: 1495: 1492: 1491: 1489: 1485: 1479: 1476: 1474: 1471: 1469: 1466: 1464: 1461: 1459: 1456: 1454: 1451: 1449: 1446: 1444: 1441: 1439: 1436: 1434: 1431: 1429: 1426: 1424: 1421: 1419: 1416: 1414: 1413:Konrad Lorenz 1411: 1409: 1406: 1404: 1401: 1399: 1396: 1395: 1393: 1389: 1382: 1379: 1377: 1374: 1372: 1369: 1367: 1364: 1362: 1359: 1357: 1354: 1352: 1349: 1347: 1346:NMDA receptor 1344: 1342: 1339: 1337: 1334: 1332: 1329: 1327: 1326: 1322: 1320: 1317: 1315: 1312: 1311: 1309: 1305: 1301: 1300:Neuroethology 1294: 1289: 1287: 1282: 1280: 1275: 1274: 1271: 1265: 1262: 1257: 1253: 1250: 1246: 1243: 1242: 1240: 1237: 1236: 1232: 1227: 1224: 1220: 1217: 1213: 1208: 1205: 1201: 1197: 1193: 1189: 1185: 1178: 1173: 1172: 1168: 1160: 1157: 1156: 1152: 1149: 1143: 1140: 1136: 1132: 1127: 1122: 1118: 1114: 1110: 1106: 1099: 1096: 1092: 1088: 1084: 1080: 1073: 1066: 1063: 1059: 1055: 1050: 1045: 1041: 1037: 1033: 1026: 1024: 1020: 1016: 1012: 1008: 1004: 1000: 996: 989: 986: 982: 978: 974: 970: 966: 962: 955: 953: 949: 945: 939: 934: 933: 924: 921: 917: 913: 909: 905: 901: 897: 890: 883: 881: 879: 877: 875: 871: 867: 861: 857: 856:Neuroethology 850: 848: 846: 844: 840: 830:on 2007-06-11 826: 822: 818: 814: 810: 806: 802: 798: 794: 787: 780: 778: 776: 774: 772: 770: 766: 762: 758: 754: 750: 746: 742: 735: 732: 728: 724: 720: 716: 713:(4): 509–25, 712: 708: 701: 699: 695: 691: 687: 683: 679: 675: 671: 667: 660: 658: 656: 654: 650: 647: 643: 640: 636: 630: 626: 619: 617: 615: 613: 611: 609: 607: 605: 603: 601: 599: 597: 595: 593: 591: 589: 587: 585: 583: 581: 579: 577: 573: 568: 561: 559: 557: 553: 546: 542: 539: 537: 534: 532: 529: 527: 524: 522: 519: 517: 514: 512: 509: 507: 504: 503: 499: 497: 495: 491: 490: 485: 481: 480: 468: 463: 456: 454: 452: 447: 442: 438: 437:mantis shrimp 432: 425: 423: 419: 413:Social status 412: 410: 403: 401: 398: 392: 385: 383: 376: 374: 367: 365: 363: 354: 352: 350: 345: 344:muscle fibers 336: 334: 330: 326: 319: 317: 314: 310: 308: 299: 297: 293: 289: 282: 280: 276: 274: 271: 267: 259: 254: 252: 249: 244: 240: 234: 231: 230:motor neurons 227: 222: 218: 215: 211: 206: 204: 199: 195: 187: 185: 182: 177: 173: 169: 165: 164: 159: 158:neuroethology 151: 149: 147: 142: 140: 136: 132: 128: 123: 119: 118: 113: 106: 105: 99: 92: 90: 88: 83: 79: 74: 72: 68: 64: 60: 56: 53: 50: 46: 42: 38: 37:tail-flipping 34: 30: 21: 1601: 1589: 1570:Lateral line 1544: 1520:Waggle dance 1458:Eric Knudsen 1323: 1225: 1222: 1215: 1211: 1187: 1183: 1158: 1146: 1142: 1108: 1104: 1098: 1082: 1078: 1065: 1039: 1035: 998: 994: 988: 964: 960: 931: 923: 899: 895: 855: 832:, retrieved 825:the original 796: 792: 744: 740: 734: 710: 706: 676:(1): 23–38, 673: 669: 624: 566: 493: 487: 483: 477: 461: 460: 445: 440: 433: 429: 420: 416: 407: 393: 389: 380: 371: 358: 355:3 – Swimming 340: 331: 327: 323: 315: 311: 303: 294: 290: 286: 277: 263: 235: 226:motor nerves 207: 191: 167: 161: 155: 145: 143: 122:interneurons 115: 109: 102: 75: 36: 32: 28: 26: 1494:Patch clamp 1463:Eric Kandel 1443:Franz Huber 1314:Feedforward 967:(1): 3–15, 902:(1): 1–18, 198:exoskeleton 192:Like other 55:crustaceans 1619:Categories 1468:Nobuo Suga 1383:in insects 1228:: 145–172. 1218:: 761–781. 834:2008-11-10 547:References 451:homologous 349:hair cells 168:P. clarkii 49:freshwater 33:lobstering 761:143708768 457:Etymology 266:dendrites 110:In 1946, 78:abdominal 1591:Category 1331:Instinct 1307:Concepts 1258:anatomy. 1151:Archived 1058:15107440 981:41122772 916:21046844 813:10203852 747:: 3–39, 727:14206963 690:20279137 642:Archived 506:Crayfish 500:See also 462:Caridoid 404:Learning 214:extensor 71:crayfish 59:lobsters 57:such as 1603:Commons 1508:Systems 1487:Methods 1256:decapod 1204:6033993 1159:ΛΟΓΕΙΟΝ 1135:8410194 1126:6576376 1015:6090603 821:1197778 569:: 10–45 489:Caridea 446:Squilla 441:Squilla 439:called 273:stimuli 270:tactile 221:ganglia 217:muscles 194:decapod 87:neurons 82:thrusts 52:eucarid 1391:People 1325:Umwelt 1202:  1133:  1123:  1056:  1013:  979:  940:  914:  862:  819:  811:  759:  725:  688:  631:  479:shrimp 377:Eating 210:flexor 203:telson 181:telson 67:shrimp 45:marine 1180:(PDF) 1148:καρίς 1075:(PDF) 977:S2CID 892:(PDF) 828:(PDF) 817:S2CID 789:(PDF) 757:S2CID 484:prawn 473:καρίς 467:karis 243:axons 63:krill 1200:PMID 1131:PMID 1054:PMID 1011:PMID 938:ISBN 912:PMID 860:ISBN 809:PMID 723:PMID 686:PMID 629:ISBN 212:and 69:and 47:and 27:The 1226:121 1192:doi 1121:PMC 1113:doi 1087:doi 1083:112 1044:doi 1040:207 1003:doi 969:doi 965:153 904:doi 801:doi 749:doi 715:doi 678:doi 482:or 470:or 43:in 35:or 1621:: 1216:47 1214:. 1198:, 1188:46 1186:, 1182:, 1129:, 1119:, 1109:13 1107:, 1081:, 1077:, 1052:, 1038:, 1034:, 1022:^ 1009:, 999:52 997:, 975:, 963:, 951:^ 910:, 900:56 898:, 894:, 873:^ 842:^ 815:, 807:, 797:22 795:, 791:, 768:^ 755:, 743:, 721:, 711:12 709:, 697:^ 684:, 674:10 672:, 668:, 652:^ 575:^ 555:^ 496:. 453:. 89:. 73:. 65:, 61:, 1292:e 1285:t 1278:v 1194:: 1115:: 1089:: 1046:: 1005:: 971:: 906:: 803:: 751:: 745:1 717:: 680::

Index


escape behavior
marine
freshwater
eucarid
crustaceans
lobsters
krill
shrimp
crayfish
abdominal
thrusts
neurons

Procambarus clarkii
C. A. G. Wiersma
Procambarus clarkii
interneurons
lateral giant interneuron
medial giant interneuron
command neuron
depolarization
neuroethology
Procambarus clarkii
fixed action pattern
escape response
telson
decapod
exoskeleton
telson

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.