Knowledge (XXG)

Closure (psychology)

Source πŸ“

96:
factors, decisiveness and need for structure. Thus, using a total scale score can overlook effects for each factor and complicate interpretations. In 2007, Roets and Van Hiel tried to resolve this issue by revising the scale so it would measure only one thing. They came up with a set of new decisiveness items that provided a viable alternative for the old Decisiveness subscale of the NFCS, which was poorly related to the other NFCS facet scales and had questionable validity. The new items were developed with explicit reference to decisiveness but formulated in such a way that they relate to the need rather than to the ability to decide. In 2011, Roets and Van Hiel created an abridged and empirically validated NFC scale consisting of only 15 items from the original NFC.
149:
action, or social status accorded the possessors of knowledge (i.e., "experts"). Similarly, some perceived costs of lacking closure may relate to the additional time and effort required to attain closure, or the unpleasantness of process whereby closure must be reached. Occasionally, however, lack of closure may be perceived to offer various advantages such as freedom from a constraining commitment, neutrality in an acrimonious dispute, the maintenance of a romantic mystery and so on. Though lack of closure is generally thought of as being negative, it is clear that closure and lack of closure have positive or negative implications depending on the person and situation surrounding them.
161:, etc.). Ideally, people should attempt to acquire new knowledge to satisfy questions regarding particular issues (specific cognitive closure) irrespective of whether that knowledge points to a conclusion having positive or negative implications for them (non-specific cognitive closure). But because urgency and permanence are central to the motivational core of this overall process, individuals (or groups) may be compelled, consciously or unconsciously, to obtain information prematurely and irrespective of content. 242:"Closure" has also been used more loosely to refer to the outcome of an experience which, by virtue of its completion, demonstrates a therapeutic value. Legal scholars have linked "closure" to "catharsis" and "satisfaction" and at times the legal system may be enlisted into an individual's desire for the cessation of uncertainty. In the case of the death penalty, for example, victims seeking "closure" may adopt effective strategies as diverse as retribution, on one hand, and forgiveness on the other. 211:
ideas, even if those ideas or knowledge are distinctly unrelated to any specific content or information being presented in the classroom. In instances such as these, an individual's desire for cognitive closure in another area may outweigh her/his motivation to expend cognitive resources toward learning new information. As a result, the student may appear uninterested and susceptible to under-achieving e.g. poor grades or not performing to expected levels.
207:
readiness, civic engagement, and general well-being. However, although it is understood that the basic principles of learning assert the importance of attending to students' prior knowledge, fostering conceptual understanding, and cultivating metacognitive awareness, students must also become engaged and be willing to tolerate and cognitively work through the intellectual ambiguity often associated with exposure to novel information and tasks.
68:., need for closure exerts its effects via two general tendencies: the urgency tendency (the inclination to attain closure as quickly as possible) and the permanence tendency (the tendency to maintain it for as long as possible). Together, these tendencies may produce the inclinations to seize and then freeze on early judgmental cues, reducing the extent of information processing and hypothesis generation and introducing biases in thinking. 215:
deficiency that is impeding the learning process. This is not to suggest that need for cognitive closure is a suitable explanation for all learning problems; however, in working with students who appear to be experiencing learning challenges manifested through amotivation or low motivation, it would not be unreasonable to explore need for cognitive closure as a potential factor.
92:
statements such as "I think that having clear rules and order at work is essential to success," and "I do not like situations that are uncertain." Items such as "Even after I've made up my mind about something, I am always eager to consider a different opinion," and "I like to have friends who are unpredictable" are reverse scored.
182:
alternatives on one attribute and then moving on to the next attribute. Individuals with a lower NFCC, in contrast, used the "alternative-based search", such that they examine all attributes of one alternative, then move on to the next alternative. Thus, studying NFCC has huge implications for consumer buying behavior.
57:, and the hoped-for benefits, such as a sense of relief after the death of a person who inflicted harm, are not necessarily obtained. Because of this mismatch between what individuals hope will happen if they achieve closure and what they actually experience, the idea of getting closure has been described as a myth. 235:, individuals with high need-for-closure ratings had low creativity scores. Those low in need-for-closure more frequently produced novel solutions that motivated and inspired others in their groups, and the outcomes of the projects in which they participated were rated as correspondingly more productive. 136:
The need to avoid closure may stem from the perceived costs of possessing closure (e.g., envisioned penalties for an erroneous closure or perceived drawbacks of actions implied by closure) and the perceived benefits of lacking closure (e.g., immunity from possible criticism of any given closure). The
48:
The term "need" denotes a motivated tendency to seek out information. The need for closure is the motivation to find an answer to an ambiguous situation. This motivation is enhanced by the perceived benefits of obtaining closure, such as the increased ability to predict the world and a stronger basis
148:
The lack of closure leaves a situation in ambiguity. People high in need for closure seek to avoid this ambiguity at all costs where people high in need to avoid closure strive to make situations more ambiguous. Some perceived benefits of cognitive closure may relate to predictability, the basis for
238:
Most research on the need for closure has investigated its relation to social stimuli. However, recent research suggests that it may also predict responses to non-social stimuli. In particular, the need for closure predicts an evaluative bias against deviant non-social stimuli (e.g., the letter "A"
210:
Yet for students who have high need for cognitive closure, this phenomenon may inadvertently lead to the inhibition of cognitive functions and processes essential to the learning process, so that they can maintain their prior certainty and/or perceived permanence of personally or socially important
185:
Need for closure has also been found to have a role in race- and gender-based prejudice. Roets describes a conceptual fit between Allport's "motivated cognitive style" of individuals who exhibit prejudice and Kruglanksi and Webster's concept of high-NFCC individuals, such that both display urgency
181:
For example, the level of NFCC can influence decision-making strategies used by an individual. In a study by Choi et al. that manipulated NFCC, the authors found that a higher NFCC was associated with a preference for using the faster "attribute-based search" which involves examining all available
60:
The level of the need for cognitive closure is a fairly stable individual characteristic. It can affect what information individuals seek out and how they process it. This need can be affected by situational factors. For example, in the presence of circumstances that increase the need for closure,
206:
Formal education environments, such as elementary and secondary schools, present opportunities for learners to acquire new knowledge and skills, and to achieve deep, domain-specific conceptual mastery which, through well-designed pedagogical guidance and academic study, may enhance future career
193:
Additionally, and especially in those with strong needs for certainty (as measured on NFC Scale), the impulse to achieve cognitive closure may sometimes produce or evoke a mood instability, and/or truncated perceptions of one's available behavioral choices, should some newly acquired information
95:
Composed of 42 items, the scale has been used in numerous research studies and has been translated into multiple languages. Although Webster and Kruglanski (1994) treated the Need for Closure Scale as unidimensional (i.e., as measuring a single factor), the scale actually contains two orthogonal
91:
to correspondence bias, make stereotypical judgments, assimilate new information to existing, active beliefs, and, in the presence of prior information, resist persuasion. Someone rating low on need for closure will express more ideational fluidity and creative acts. Items on the scale include
214:
In the absence of understanding and consideration of how need for cognitive closure may influence academic and/or achievement motivation, educators may erroneously conclude that a student does not have a desire to learn or that she/he has a cognitive, psychological, intellectual, or behavioral
189:
A high need also induces the tendency to form knowledge more quickly, tying into other concepts, such as a tendency to prefer autocracy i.e. "hard" forms of influence that motivate the targets to comply with the agents' demands quickly via the promise of positive consequences or the threat of
132:
Functionally opposite to the need for closure is the need to avoid closure. Need to avoid closure reflects the desire to suspend judgmental commitment. It also contains the subcategories specific and non-specific need to avoid closure. Avoidance of specific closure reflects the desire to avoid
223:
Individuals scoring high on the NFCS are more likely to attempt to draw closure by relying on incipient cues, and the first-encountered apparent fit. The need for closure is also said to predispose a very narrow or shallow information search, along with a higher tendency to use cognitive
186:
tendency i.e. the desire for quick, definite answers and permanence tendency i.e., the perseverance of the obtained answer in spite of contradictory information. Thus, NFC provides a strong empirical base for Allport's hypothesized underlying cognitive style of prejudiced individuals.
133:
specific answers to one's questions. The non-specific need to avoid closure is much like the need for closure irrespective of whether or not this new knowledge points to a conclusion having positive or negative implications for them.
197:
Thus it is apparent that the need for cognitive closure may have important implications for both personal and inter-personal thoughts and actions, including some related to educational processes and school learning.
84:, Donna Webster, and Adena Klem in 1993 and is designed to operationalize this construct and is presented as a unidimensional instrument possessing strong discriminant and predictive validity. 137:
need to avoid closure is controlled by the desire to avoid negative consequences of achieving closure of a situation or to continue the benefits of not closing but elongating a situation.
960:
Pierro, A.; Kruglanski, A. W.; Raven, B. H. (February 2012). "Motivational underpinnings of social influence in work settings: Bases of social power and the need for cognitive closure".
157:
A need for cognitive closure may occur while engaged in goal-driven or goal-motivated cognitive functions (e.g., attention control, memory recall, information selection and processing,
140:
The need and avoidance of closure are conceptualized as ends of a continuum ranging from strong strivings for closure to strong resistance of closure. This is applied in the NFC Scale.
1218: 545:
Neuberg, S. L.; Judice, T.; West, S. G. (June 1997). "What the Need for Closure Scale measures and what it does not: Toward differentiating among related epistemic motives".
80:
is thought to be a fairly stable dispositional characteristic that can, nonetheless, be affected by situational factors. The Need for Closure Scale (NFCS) was developed by
49:
for action. This motivation is also enhanced by the perceived costs of lacking closure, such as dealing with uncertainty. A sense of closure is not usually possible with
45:), are social psychological terms that describe an individual's desire for a clear, firm answer or peaceful resolution to a question or problem to avert ambiguity. 415: 612:
Kruglanski, A. W.; Webster, D. M.; Klem, A (November 1993). "Motivated resistance and openness to persuasion in the presence or absence of prior information".
577:
Chirumbolo, A.; Livi, S.; Mannetti, L.; Pierro, A.; Kruglanski, A. (June 2004). "Effects of Need for Closure on Creativity in Small Group Interactions".
194:
challenge preconceptions that they had long considered to be certain, permanent and inviolate e.g. certain religious or ethical views and values.
118:
High NFC scores consistently correlate with items on the C-Scale (conservatism) as well as other measures of political and social conservatism.
1022:
Harlow, L.; Debacker, T. K.; Crowson, M. H. (2011). "Need for Closure, Achievement Goals, and Cognitive Engagement in High School Students".
440: 391: 690:
Roets, A.; Van Hiel, A (February 2007). "Separating ability from need: Clarifying the dimensional structure of the need for closure scale".
1264: 1059: 1095: 987:
DeBacker, T. K.; Crowson, H. M. (September 2006). "Influences on cognitive engagement: epistemological beliefs and need for closure".
1233: 499:
Van Hiel, A.; Mervielde, I. (2003). "The Need for Closure and the Spontaneous Use of Complex and Simple Cognitive Structures".
647:
Stalder, D. R (April 2012). "Investigation of the two-factor model for the English version of the Need for Closure Scale".
857:
Webster, Donna M.; Kruglanski, Arie W (1997). "Cognitive and Social Consequences of the Need for Cognitive Closure".
884:
Choi, J.; Koo, M.; Choi, I.; Auh, S. (November 2008). "Need for cognitive closure and information search strategy".
190:
negative consequences, rather than "soft" forms of influence that might use extended argumentation or persuasion.
775:"Examination of Psychometric Properties of the Need for Closure Scale-Short Form among Turkish College Students" 20: 1113:"The relationship between the need for closure and deviant bias: An investigation of generality and process" 294:
Kruglanski, A. W.; Webster, D. M. (April 1996). "Motivated closing of the mind: 'Seizing' and 'freezing'".
1259: 913:"Allport's Prejudiced Personality Today: Need for Closure as the Motivated Cognitive Basis of Prejudice" 158: 112: 342:
Webster, D.; Kruglanski, A (December 1994). "Individual differences in need for cognitive closure".
171:
initiating and sustaining cognitive manipulations that are required to achieve particular outcomes
1224: 1039: 942: 715: 672: 594: 524: 409: 319: 257: 168:
selecting the most relevant information one should attend to for increasing chances of adaptation
127: 81: 1057:
DeBacker, T. K.; Crowson, H (October 2008). "Measuring need for closure in classroom learners".
1142: 1091: 1004: 812: 794: 707: 664: 629: 516: 481: 446: 436: 397: 387: 359: 311: 77: 1132: 1124: 1068: 1031: 996: 969: 932: 924: 893: 866: 839: 802: 786: 755: 699: 656: 621: 586: 554: 508: 473: 351: 303: 100: 1237: 87:
People who score high on the need for closure scale are more likely to exhibit impression
807: 774: 660: 269: 88: 61:
individuals are more likely to use simple cognitive structures to process information.
54: 50: 1253: 1184: 1043: 743: 598: 1230: 946: 719: 528: 323: 1161: 1072: 676: 1128: 830:
Kruglanski, A. W. (1990). "Lay Epistemic Theory in Social-Cognitive Psychology".
732:
Jost, Kruglanski et al 2003, Political Conservatism As Motivated Social Cognition
1213: 759: 558: 355: 307: 843: 625: 1035: 912: 870: 512: 401: 232: 928: 798: 703: 450: 1208: 1185:"Capital Punishment as 'Closure': Limits of a Victim-Centered Jurisprudence" 1000: 263: 225: 108: 104: 1146: 1008: 937: 816: 790: 744:"Classic and Current Social Comparison Research: Expanding the Perspective" 711: 668: 520: 485: 381: 1137: 633: 430: 363: 315: 477: 897: 973: 464:
Boss, Pauline; Carnes, Donna (December 2012). "The Myth of Closure".
1242: 590: 251: 254: β€“ Belief(s) accepted by members of a group without question 115:, among several other cognitive tools and personality traits. 1112: 1086:
Bransford, J. D.; Brown, A. L.; Cocking, R. R., eds. (2000).
164:
A high need for cognitive closure might then invite bias in:
16:
Psychological term for one's need for an answer to something
432:
Killing McVeigh: The Death Penalty and the Myth of Closure
383:
The Myth of Closure: Ambiguous Loss in a Time of Pandemic
228:, when seeking solutions. (Van Hiel and Mervielde, 2003) 177:
weighing information during the course of decision-making
289: 287: 285: 1088:
How people learn: Brain, mind, experience, and school
174:
making judgments and assessments of input information
1189:
New York University Review of Law and Social Change
1162:"Victims, 'Closure,' and the Sociology of Emotion" 337: 335: 333: 773:ATAK, Hasan; SYED, Moin; Γ‡OK, Figen (June 2017). 1209:Robert Fulford's column about the word "closure" 572: 570: 568: 911:Roets, Arne; Van Hiel, Alain (December 2011). 111:and negatively with cognitive complexity and 8: 1111:Rubin, M.; Paolini, S; Crisp, R. J. (2011). 1090:. Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press. 614:Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 547:Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 344:Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 917:Current Directions in Psychological Science 692:Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 414:: CS1 maint: location missing publisher ( 1136: 989:British Journal of Educational Psychology 936: 806: 742:Mayseless, O.; Kruglanski, A. W. (1990). 540: 538: 239:presented in a category of letter "B"s) 435:. New York: New York University Press. 281: 407: 962:European Journal of Social Psychology 99:NFCS items correlate positively with 7: 1243:Gestalt Therapy / Gestalt Psychology 859:European Review of Social Psychology 375: 373: 1117:International Journal of Psychology 1060:Contemporary Educational Psychology 1024:The Journal of Educational Research 1214:Comments on the Concept of Closure 661:10.2466/03.09.20.PR0.110.2.598-606 14: 1160:Bandes, Susan A. (Spring 2009). 501:The Journal of Social Psychology 386:(First ed.). New York, NY. 579:European Journal of Personality 1073:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2007.06.001 266: β€“ Problem-solving method 1: 1166:Law and Contemporary Problems 37:), used interchangeably with 1129:10.1080/00207594.2010.537660 429:Madeira, Jody LyneΓ© (2012). 103:, intolerance of ambiguity, 779:Archives of Neuropsychiatry 760:10.1037/0033-2909.108.2.195 559:10.1037/0022-3514.72.6.1396 356:10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.1049 308:10.1037/0033-295X.103.2.263 1281: 1265:Social psychology concepts 886:Psychology & Marketing 844:10.1207/s15327965pli0103_1 626:10.1037/0022-3514.65.5.861 260: β€“ Psychology concept 125: 39:need for cognitive closure 18: 1183:Kanwar, Vik (2001–2002). 1036:10.1080/00220670903567406 871:10.1080/14792779643000100 513:10.1080/00224540309598463 1231:Sensation and Perception 929:10.1177/0963721411424894 704:10.1177/0146167206294744 76:The need for closure in 64:According to Kruglanski 21:Closure (disambiguation) 1001:10.1348/000709905X53138 791:10.5152/npa.2017.12707 748:Psychological Bulletin 380:Boss, Pauline (2022). 272: β€“ Cognitive bias 72:Need for Closure Scale 832:Psychological Inquiry 649:Psychological Reports 122:Need to avoid closure 107:, need for order and 995:(535–551): 535–551. 296:Psychological Review 159:cognitive inhibition 19:For other uses, see 1236:2011-07-07 at the 1225:Gestalt psychology 1219:Sorrow and Closure 478:10.1111/famp.12005 258:Need for cognition 128:Need for cognition 898:10.1002/mar.20253 892:(11): 1027–1042. 442:978-0-8147-2454-5 393:978-1-324-01681-6 78:social psychology 1272: 1197: 1196: 1180: 1174: 1173: 1157: 1151: 1150: 1140: 1108: 1102: 1101: 1083: 1077: 1076: 1054: 1048: 1047: 1019: 1013: 1012: 984: 978: 977: 974:10.1002/ejsp.836 957: 951: 950: 940: 908: 902: 901: 881: 875: 874: 854: 848: 847: 827: 821: 820: 810: 770: 764: 763: 739: 733: 730: 724: 723: 687: 681: 680: 644: 638: 637: 609: 603: 602: 574: 563: 562: 553:(6): 1396–1412. 542: 533: 532: 496: 490: 489: 461: 455: 454: 426: 420: 419: 413: 405: 377: 368: 367: 339: 328: 327: 291: 101:authoritarianism 31:need for closure 1280: 1279: 1275: 1274: 1273: 1271: 1270: 1269: 1250: 1249: 1238:Wayback Machine 1205: 1200: 1182: 1181: 1177: 1159: 1158: 1154: 1110: 1109: 1105: 1098: 1085: 1084: 1080: 1056: 1055: 1051: 1021: 1020: 1016: 986: 985: 981: 959: 958: 954: 938:1854/LU-2109466 910: 909: 905: 883: 882: 878: 856: 855: 851: 829: 828: 824: 772: 771: 767: 741: 740: 736: 731: 727: 689: 688: 684: 646: 645: 641: 611: 610: 606: 591:10.1002/per.518 576: 575: 566: 544: 543: 536: 498: 497: 493: 463: 462: 458: 443: 428: 427: 423: 406: 394: 379: 378: 371: 341: 340: 331: 293: 292: 283: 279: 248: 221: 204: 155: 146: 144:Lack of closure 130: 124: 89:primacy effects 82:Arie Kruglanski 74: 24: 17: 12: 11: 5: 1278: 1276: 1268: 1267: 1262: 1252: 1251: 1248: 1247: 1246: 1245: 1240: 1221: 1216: 1211: 1204: 1203:External links 1201: 1199: 1198: 1175: 1152: 1138:1959.13/930915 1123:(3): 206–213. 1103: 1097:978-0309065573 1096: 1078: 1067:(4): 711–732. 1049: 1030:(2): 110–119. 1014: 979: 952: 923:(6): 349–354. 903: 876: 865:(1): 133–173. 849: 838:(3): 181–197. 822: 785:(2): 175–182. 765: 754:(2): 195–208. 734: 725: 682: 655:(2): 598–606. 639: 604: 564: 534: 491: 472:(4): 456–469. 466:Family Process 456: 441: 421: 392: 369: 350:(6): 1049–62. 329: 280: 278: 275: 274: 273: 270:Zero-risk bias 267: 261: 255: 247: 244: 231:In studies on 220: 217: 203: 200: 179: 178: 175: 172: 169: 154: 151: 145: 142: 126:Main article: 123: 120: 73: 70: 55:missing person 51:ambiguous loss 15: 13: 10: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 1277: 1266: 1263: 1261: 1260:Psychotherapy 1258: 1257: 1255: 1244: 1241: 1239: 1235: 1232: 1229: 1228: 1226: 1222: 1220: 1217: 1215: 1212: 1210: 1207: 1206: 1202: 1194: 1190: 1186: 1179: 1176: 1171: 1167: 1163: 1156: 1153: 1148: 1144: 1139: 1134: 1130: 1126: 1122: 1118: 1114: 1107: 1104: 1099: 1093: 1089: 1082: 1079: 1074: 1070: 1066: 1062: 1061: 1053: 1050: 1045: 1041: 1037: 1033: 1029: 1025: 1018: 1015: 1010: 1006: 1002: 998: 994: 990: 983: 980: 975: 971: 967: 963: 956: 953: 948: 944: 939: 934: 930: 926: 922: 918: 914: 907: 904: 899: 895: 891: 887: 880: 877: 872: 868: 864: 860: 853: 850: 845: 841: 837: 833: 826: 823: 818: 814: 809: 804: 800: 796: 792: 788: 784: 780: 776: 769: 766: 761: 757: 753: 749: 745: 738: 735: 729: 726: 721: 717: 713: 709: 705: 701: 698:(2): 266–80. 697: 693: 686: 683: 678: 674: 670: 666: 662: 658: 654: 650: 643: 640: 635: 631: 627: 623: 620:(5): 861–76. 619: 615: 608: 605: 600: 596: 592: 588: 585:(4): 265–78. 584: 580: 573: 571: 569: 565: 560: 556: 552: 548: 541: 539: 535: 530: 526: 522: 518: 514: 510: 507:(5): 559–68. 506: 502: 495: 492: 487: 483: 479: 475: 471: 467: 460: 457: 452: 448: 444: 438: 434: 433: 425: 422: 417: 411: 403: 399: 395: 389: 385: 384: 376: 374: 370: 365: 361: 357: 353: 349: 345: 338: 336: 334: 330: 325: 321: 317: 313: 309: 305: 302:(2): 263–83. 301: 297: 290: 288: 286: 282: 276: 271: 268: 265: 262: 259: 256: 253: 250: 249: 245: 243: 240: 236: 234: 229: 227: 218: 216: 212: 208: 201: 199: 195: 191: 187: 183: 176: 173: 170: 167: 166: 165: 162: 160: 152: 150: 143: 141: 138: 134: 129: 121: 119: 116: 114: 110: 106: 102: 97: 93: 90: 85: 83: 79: 71: 69: 67: 62: 58: 56: 52: 46: 44: 40: 36: 32: 28: 22: 1192: 1188: 1178: 1169: 1165: 1155: 1120: 1116: 1106: 1087: 1081: 1064: 1058: 1052: 1027: 1023: 1017: 992: 988: 982: 968:(1): 41–52. 965: 961: 955: 920: 916: 906: 889: 885: 879: 862: 858: 852: 835: 831: 825: 782: 778: 768: 751: 747: 737: 728: 695: 691: 685: 652: 648: 642: 617: 613: 607: 582: 578: 550: 546: 504: 500: 494: 469: 465: 459: 431: 424: 382: 347: 343: 299: 295: 241: 237: 230: 222: 213: 209: 205: 202:In education 196: 192: 188: 184: 180: 163: 156: 153:Implications 147: 139: 135: 131: 117: 98: 94: 86: 75: 65: 63: 59: 53:, such as a 47: 42: 38: 34: 30: 26: 25: 1223:Closure in 252:Dogmaticism 113:impulsivity 1254:Categories 1172:(1): 1–26. 402:1249706143 277:References 233:creativity 226:heuristics 1044:145178984 799:1300-0667 599:144190667 451:794003582 410:cite book 264:Heuristic 109:structure 105:dogmatism 1234:Archived 1147:22044233 1009:16953961 947:96427663 817:28680317 720:34733882 712:17259586 669:22662413 529:41445636 521:14609052 486:23230978 324:19253040 246:See also 219:Research 808:5491669 677:8345812 634:8246114 364:7815301 316:8637961 27:Closure 1145:  1094:  1042:  1007:  945:  815:  805:  797:  718:  710:  675:  667:  632:  597:  527:  519:  484:  449:  439:  400:  390:  362:  322:  314:  1040:S2CID 943:S2CID 716:S2CID 673:S2CID 595:S2CID 525:S2CID 320:S2CID 66:et al 1143:PMID 1092:ISBN 1005:PMID 813:PMID 795:ISSN 708:PMID 665:PMID 630:PMID 517:PMID 482:PMID 447:OCLC 437:ISBN 416:link 398:OCLC 388:ISBN 360:PMID 312:PMID 43:NFCC 1133:hdl 1125:doi 1069:doi 1032:doi 1028:104 997:doi 970:doi 933:hdl 925:doi 894:doi 867:doi 840:doi 803:PMC 787:doi 756:doi 752:108 700:doi 657:doi 653:110 622:doi 587:doi 555:doi 509:doi 505:143 474:doi 352:doi 304:doi 300:103 35:NFC 29:or 1256:: 1227:: 1193:27 1191:. 1187:. 1170:72 1168:. 1164:. 1141:. 1131:. 1121:46 1119:. 1115:. 1065:33 1063:. 1038:. 1026:. 1003:. 993:76 991:. 966:42 964:. 941:. 931:. 921:20 919:. 915:. 890:25 888:. 861:. 834:. 811:. 801:. 793:. 783:54 781:. 777:. 750:. 746:. 714:. 706:. 696:33 694:. 671:. 663:. 651:. 628:. 618:65 616:. 593:. 583:18 581:. 567:^ 551:72 549:. 537:^ 523:. 515:. 503:. 480:. 470:51 468:. 445:. 412:}} 408:{{ 396:. 372:^ 358:. 348:67 346:. 332:^ 318:. 310:. 298:. 284:^ 1195:. 1149:. 1135:: 1127:: 1100:. 1075:. 1071:: 1046:. 1034:: 1011:. 999:: 976:. 972:: 949:. 935:: 927:: 900:. 896:: 873:. 869:: 863:8 846:. 842:: 836:1 819:. 789:: 762:. 758:: 722:. 702:: 679:. 659:: 636:. 624:: 601:. 589:: 561:. 557:: 531:. 511:: 488:. 476:: 453:. 418:) 404:. 366:. 354:: 326:. 306:: 41:( 33:( 23:.

Index

Closure (disambiguation)
ambiguous loss
missing person
social psychology
Arie Kruglanski
primacy effects
authoritarianism
dogmatism
structure
impulsivity
Need for cognition
cognitive inhibition
heuristics
creativity
Dogmaticism
Need for cognition
Heuristic
Zero-risk bias



doi
10.1037/0033-295X.103.2.263
PMID
8637961
S2CID
19253040


Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.

↑