Knowledge (XXG)

Hundredth monkey effect

Source đź“ť

259:, applying it to human society and the effecting of positive change. Unfortunately, Keyes combined two items of truth: that the Koshima monkeys learned to wash sweet potatoes, and that the phenomenon was observed on neighboring islands. He did not provide substantiating evidence for his claims, diluting the importance of both studies and potentially discrediting the scientists involved. Combining this science with his political views may also have damaged the research credibility, leading to many reporters attempting to 'debunk' the Japanese team's research without doing sufficient research themselves. 363:, revealed several key points that demystified the supposed effect. Claims that the practice spread suddenly to other isolated populations of monkeys may be called into question given the fact that the monkeys had the researchers in common. Amundson also notes that the sweet potato was not available to the monkeys prior to human intervention. Moreover, the number of monkeys in the colony was counted as 59 in 1962, indicating that even in numbers no "hundredth monkey" existed. 141: 367:
is widespread in the animal kingdom; older monkeys who did not know how to wash tended not to learn. As the older monkeys died and younger monkeys were born the proportion of washers naturally increased. The time span between observations by the Japanese scientists was on the order of years so the increase in the proportion was not observed to be sudden.
279:
sweet potatoes. No mention of the other behavioral improvements are made and no indication of how the monkey swam—the Koshima monkeys cannot swim. Therefore, although the question must be asked how the swimming monkey learned the sweet potato washing behavior if not from Koshima, no indication is made as to where the monkey learned the behavior.
366:
Unsubstantiated claims that there was a sudden and remarkable increase in the proportion of washers in the first population were exaggerations of a much slower, more mundane effect. Rather than all monkeys mysteriously learning the skill, it was noted that it was predominantly a learned skill, which
229:
The innovator's sibling started another innovation whereas the monkeys were initially fearful of the ocean, only deigning to put their hands and feet into it, the wheat straining innovation led to monkeys submerging more of their bodies in the water, or play-splashing in the ocean. This behavior was
225:
The first innovator continues to innovate. The young monkey who started potato washing also learned how to sift wheat grains out of the sand by throwing handfuls of sand and wheat into the water, then catching the wheat that floated to the top. This invention was also copied using the above teaching
77:
in open areas, often on beaches. An unanticipated byproduct of the study was that the scientists witnessed several innovative evolutionary behavioral changes by the troop, two of which were orchestrated by one young female, and the others by her sibling or contemporaries. The account of only one of
278:
Separate papers make mention that, from 1960 onward, similar sweet potato-washing behaviors were noticed in other parts of the world, however this is not directly attributed to Koshima. Claims are made that a monkey swam from one island to another where he taught the resident monkeys how to wash
292:
were insufficient to support Watson's story. In short, she is suspicious of the existence of a 'hundredth monkey' phenomenon; the published articles describe how the sweet potato-washing behavior gradually spread through the monkey troop and became part of the set of learned behaviors of young
23:
is an esoteric idea claiming that a new behavior or idea is spread rapidly by unexplained means from one group to all related groups once a critical number of members of one group exhibit the new behavior or acknowledge the new idea. The behavior was said to propagate even to groups that are
101:
had suddenly developed among the monkeys, as a result of one last monkey learning potato washing by conventional means (rather than the one-monkey-at-a-time method prior). Watson concluded that the researchers observed that, once a critical number of monkeys was reached—i.e., the hundredth
226:
and learning process until there were too many monkeys on the island with too little wheat apportioned, which is when competition became too fierce and the stronger monkeys would steal the collected wheat from the weaker ones, so they stopped the learned behavior in self-preservation.
234:
The study does not indicate a catalyst ratio at which all the Koshima monkeys started washing sweet potatoes, or a correlation to other monkey studies where similar behavior started. To the contrary, it indicated that certain age groups in Koshima would not learn the behavior.
201:
The original Koshima research was undertaken by a team of scientists as a secondary consequence of 1948 research on semi-wild monkeys in Japan. The Koshima troop was identified as segregated from other monkeys and, from 1950, used as a closed study group to observe wild
210:
Her changed behavior led to several feeding behavior changes over the course of the next few years, all of which was of great benefit in understanding the process of teaching and learning in animal behavior. A brief account of the behavioral changes can be seen below:
221:
Once the initial group have children, a change occurs in the dynamic of the behavior from teaching previous and current generations, to a new dynamic where the next generation learns by observation. The behavior is no longer actively taught but passively observed and
206:
behavior. While studying the group, the team would drop sweet potatoes and wheat on the beach and observe the troop's behavior. In 1954, a paper was published indicating the first observances of one monkey, Imo, washing her sweet potatoes in the water.
81:
According to Watson, the scientists observed that some of the monkeys learned to wash sweet potatoes, initially through an 18-month-old female member (named "Imo" by the researchers) of the troop in 1953. Imo discovered that
86:
and grit could be removed from the potatoes by washing them in a stream or in the ocean. Gradually, this new potato-washing habit spread through the troop—in the usual fashion, through
267:
In many cases of research since it was first popularized, the effect has been discredited. One of the primary factors in the spread of this concept is that many authors quote
27:
Since it was first popularized, the effect has been discredited in many cases of research. One of the primary factors in the spread of this concept is that many authors quote
659: 806: 293:
monkeys, but Myers does not agree that it serves as evidence for the existence of a critical number at which the idea suddenly spread to other islands.
162: 924: 98: 596: 840: 693: 633: 78:
these behavioral changes spread into a phenomenon (i.e., the 'hundredth monkey effect'), which Watson would then loosely publish as a story.
215:
The young first teach their contemporaries and immediate family, who all benefit from the new behavior and teach it to their contemporaries.
789: 188: 774: 349: 649: 499: 337: 325: 123: 286:. In her review, she found that the original research reports by the Japan Monkey Centre in the 2nd, 5th, and 6th volumes of 166: 890: 518: 859: 604: 479: 650:"Herding Experiment: Hundredth Monkey Phenomenon : Networks Course blog for INFO 2040/CS 2850/Econ 2040/SOC 2090" 151: 170: 155: 401: 929: 814: 94:. (Unlike most food customs, this behavior was learned by the older generation of monkeys from younger ones.) 880: 386: 914: 87: 847: 467: 218:
If the parents or their contemporaries (or their parents) are too old, they do not adopt the behavior.
728: 360: 301: 752: 654: 573: 396: 288: 91: 282:
In 1985, Elaine Myers re-examined the original published research in an article for the journal
919: 836: 785: 770: 744: 714: 689: 629: 565: 376: 103: 736: 719: 557: 313: 268: 203: 61: 28: 494: 342: 275:, or post-tertiary sources that have themselves misrepresented the original observations. 272: 252: 35:, or post-tertiary sources that have themselves misrepresented the original observations. 32: 732: 345:
explains how the urban legend started, was popularized, and has since been discredited.
420: 381: 317: 255:
on the planet. Keyes presented the 'hundredth monkey effect' story as an inspirational
114: 48: 713:; W. C. McGrew; T. Nishida; V. Reynolds; Y. Sugiyama; C. E. G. Tutin; R. W. Wrangham; 908: 321: 244: 682: 577: 756: 710: 356: 316:
has stated that a phenomenon like the hundredth monkey effect would be evidence of
305: 70: 44: 24:
physically separated and have no apparent means of communicating with each other.
59:
Between 1952 and 1953, primatologists conducted a behavioral study of a troop of
140: 898: 625: 526: 324:
and learning. As a result, the story has also become a favorite target of the
748: 569: 66: 855: 475: 43:
The 'hundredth monkey' effect was popularized in the mid-to-late 1970s by
110: 121:(1975); the story then spread with the appearance of Watson's 1979 book 97:
This behavior spread up until 1958, according to Watson, when a sort of
561: 297: 256: 597:
Senior Researcher Comments on the Hundredth Monkey Phenomenon in Japan
326:
Committee for the Scientific Investigation of Claims of the Paranormal
423:, unlike Watson, does not assign the date 1952 to the observations. 740: 74: 876: 811:
National Geographic Channel October 28, 2010/National Geographic
391: 348:
The original research continues to prove useful in the study of
309: 106:
instantly spread across the water to monkeys on nearby islands.
83: 69:. The researchers would supply these troops with such foods as 134: 230:
again copied using the above teaching and learning processes.
833:
The Hundredth Monkey—And Other Paradigms of the Paranormal
330:
The Hundredth Monkey and Other Paradigms of the Paranormal
251:(1984). Keyes's book was about the devastating effects of 548:
Galef, B. G. (1992). "The question of animal culture".
296:
The story as told by Watson and Keyes is popular among
865:
1987. "Watson and the Hundredth Monkey Phenomenon."
775:"31. Culture in primates. A - Culture as it Happens" 65:(Japanese macaque or Snow monkeys) on the island of 807:""Hot Tub Monkeys" Offer Eye on Nonhuman "Culture"" 681: 622:Rhythms of Vision: The Changing Patterns of Belief 47:, who documented the findings of several Japanese 462: 460: 458: 456: 454: 452: 450: 448: 446: 444: 442: 440: 782:The Oxford Handbook of Culture and Psychology 355:An analysis of the appropriate literature by 8: 169:. Unsourced material may be challenged and 595:Pössel, Markus, and Ron Amundson. 1996. " 189:Learn how and when to remove this message 124:Lifetide: The Biology of the Unconscious 543: 541: 539: 537: 535: 436: 413: 805:Trivedi, Bijal P. (February 6, 2004). 332:, published by the Committee in 1990. 243:This story was further popularized by 109:Watson first published the story in a 328:, and was used as the title essay in 7: 897:9(Spring 1985):10–10. Archived from 615: 613: 591: 589: 587: 525:9(Spring 1985):10–10. Archived from 513: 511: 509: 490: 488: 320:bringing about non-local effects in 167:adding citations to reliable sources 717:(1999). "Cultures in chimpanzees". 14: 877:"The Hundredth Monkey Phenomenon" 247:with the publication of his book 854:9(4):348–56. Also available via 607:from the original on 2004-08-03. 474:9(4):348–56. Also available via 350:cultural transmission in animals 139: 848:The Hundredth Monkey Phenomenon 688:. Camarillo: DeVorss & Co. 662:from the original on 2023-06-16 500:Why People Believe Weird Things 468:The Hundredth Monkey Phenomenon 338:Why People Believe Weird Things 925:Metaphors referring to monkeys 891:The Hundredth Monkey Revisited 519:The Hundredth Monkey Revisited 304:gurus, as well as becoming an 1: 875:Carroll, Robert Todd (2005). 263:Later research and criticism 946: 780:. In Jaan Valsiner (ed.). 402:Tipping point (sociology) 131:Original research (1950s) 16:Idea spreading phenomenon 620:Blair, Lawrence (1975). 889:Myers, Elaine (1985). " 387:Infinite monkey theorem 102:monkey—this previously 21:hundredth monkey effect 831:Amundson, Ron (1991). 517:Myers, Elaine. 1985. " 466:Amundson, Ron. 1985. " 882:Skeptic's Dictionary 856:University of Hawaii 835:. Prometheus Books. 784:. OUP. p. 678. 684:The Hundredth Monkey 476:University of Hawaii 361:The Skeptics Society 308:and part of New Age 249:The Hundredth Monkey 163:improve this section 733:1999Natur.399..682W 680:Keyes, Ken (1984). 99:group consciousness 867:Skeptical Inquirer 852:Skeptical Inquirer 771:Boesch, Christophe 655:Cornell University 601:Skeptical Inquirer 562:10.1007/BF02692251 472:Skeptical Inquirer 397:Multiple discovery 862:on May 25, 2011). 841:978-0-87975-655-0 727:(6737): 682–685. 695:978-0-942024-01-2 635:978-0-8052-3610-1 482:on May 25, 2011). 377:Confirmation bias 199: 198: 191: 119:Rhythms of Vision 937: 886: 819: 818: 813:. Archived from 802: 796: 795: 779: 767: 761: 760: 709:Whiten, Andrew; 706: 700: 699: 687: 677: 671: 670: 668: 667: 646: 640: 639: 617: 608: 593: 582: 581: 545: 530: 515: 504: 495:Shermer, Michael 492: 483: 464: 424: 418: 314:Rupert Sheldrake 204:Japanese macaque 194: 187: 183: 180: 174: 143: 135: 104:learned behavior 51:from the 1950s. 945: 944: 940: 939: 938: 936: 935: 934: 930:1975 neologisms 905: 904: 874: 828: 826:Further reading 823: 822: 804: 803: 799: 792: 777: 769: 768: 764: 708: 707: 703: 696: 679: 678: 674: 665: 663: 648: 647: 643: 636: 619: 618: 611: 594: 585: 547: 546: 533: 516: 507: 493: 486: 465: 438: 433: 428: 427: 419: 415: 410: 373: 359:, published by 343:Michael Shermer 302:personal-growth 265: 241: 195: 184: 178: 175: 160: 144: 133: 57: 41: 17: 12: 11: 5: 943: 941: 933: 932: 927: 922: 917: 907: 906: 903: 902: 901:on 2012-02-06. 887: 872: 871: 870: 863: 827: 824: 821: 820: 817:on 2004-04-04. 797: 790: 762: 701: 694: 672: 658:. 2021-10-29. 641: 634: 609: 583: 556:(2): 157–178. 531: 529:on 2012-02-06. 505: 484: 435: 434: 432: 429: 426: 425: 421:Lawrence Blair 412: 411: 409: 406: 405: 404: 399: 394: 389: 384: 382:Decline effect 379: 372: 369: 318:morphic fields 264: 261: 240: 237: 232: 231: 227: 223: 219: 216: 197: 196: 147: 145: 138: 132: 129: 115:Lawrence Blair 71:sweet potatoes 62:Macaca fuscata 56: 55:Watson (1970s) 53: 49:primatologists 40: 37: 15: 13: 10: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 942: 931: 928: 926: 923: 921: 918: 916: 915:Urban legends 913: 912: 910: 900: 896: 892: 888: 884: 883: 878: 873: 869:11(3):303–04. 868: 864: 861: 857: 853: 849: 845: 844: 842: 838: 834: 830: 829: 825: 816: 812: 808: 801: 798: 793: 791:9780195396430 787: 783: 776: 772: 766: 763: 758: 754: 750: 746: 742: 741:10.1038/21415 738: 734: 730: 726: 722: 721: 716: 712: 705: 702: 697: 691: 686: 685: 676: 673: 661: 657: 656: 651: 645: 642: 637: 631: 627: 623: 616: 614: 610: 606: 602: 598: 592: 590: 588: 584: 579: 575: 571: 567: 563: 559: 555: 551: 544: 542: 540: 538: 536: 532: 528: 524: 520: 514: 512: 510: 506: 502: 501: 496: 491: 489: 485: 481: 477: 473: 469: 463: 461: 459: 457: 455: 453: 451: 449: 447: 445: 443: 441: 437: 430: 422: 417: 414: 407: 403: 400: 398: 395: 393: 390: 388: 385: 383: 380: 378: 375: 374: 370: 368: 364: 362: 358: 353: 351: 346: 344: 340: 339: 333: 331: 327: 323: 322:consciousness 319: 315: 311: 307: 303: 299: 294: 291: 290: 285: 280: 276: 274: 270: 262: 260: 258: 254: 250: 246: 245:Ken Keyes Jr. 238: 236: 228: 224: 220: 217: 214: 213: 212: 208: 205: 193: 190: 182: 172: 168: 164: 158: 157: 153: 148:This section 146: 142: 137: 136: 130: 128: 126: 125: 120: 116: 112: 107: 105: 100: 95: 93: 89: 85: 79: 76: 72: 68: 64: 63: 54: 52: 50: 46: 38: 36: 34: 30: 25: 22: 899:the original 894: 881: 866: 851: 843:. Includes: 832: 815:the original 810: 800: 781: 765: 724: 718: 704: 683: 675: 664:. Retrieved 653: 644: 621: 600: 553: 550:Human Nature 549: 527:the original 522: 498: 471: 416: 365: 357:Ron Amundson 354: 347: 336: 335:In his book 334: 329: 306:urban legend 300:authors and 295: 287: 283: 281: 277: 266: 248: 242: 239:Keyes (1984) 233: 209: 200: 185: 176: 161:Please help 149: 122: 118: 108: 96: 80: 60: 58: 45:Lyall Watson 42: 26: 20: 18: 253:nuclear war 88:observation 909:Categories 895:In Context 711:J. Goodall 666:2023-06-16 626:Croom Helm 624:. London: 523:In Context 431:References 284:In Context 92:repetition 715:C. Boesch 310:mythology 269:secondary 222:mimicked. 179:July 2019 150:does not 29:secondary 920:Memetics 860:Archived 773:(2012). 749:10385119 660:Archived 605:Archived 578:15087165 570:24222403 497:. 1997. 480:Archived 371:See also 341:(1997), 289:Primates 273:tertiary 111:foreword 33:tertiary 846:1985. " 757:4385871 729:Bibcode 603:20(3). 298:New Age 257:parable 171:removed 156:sources 39:History 839:  788:  755:  747:  720:Nature 692:  632:  576:  568:  67:KĹŤjima 778:(PDF) 753:S2CID 628:Ltd. 574:S2CID 408:Notes 75:wheat 837:ISBN 786:ISBN 745:PMID 690:ISBN 630:ISBN 566:PMID 392:Meme 154:any 152:cite 90:and 84:sand 73:and 19:The 893:." 850:." 737:doi 725:399 599:." 558:doi 521:." 470:." 165:by 117:'s 113:to 911:: 879:. 809:. 751:. 743:. 735:. 723:. 652:. 612:^ 586:^ 572:. 564:. 552:. 534:^ 508:^ 487:^ 439:^ 352:. 312:. 271:, 127:. 31:, 885:. 858:( 794:. 759:. 739:: 731:: 698:. 669:. 638:. 580:. 560:: 554:3 503:. 478:( 192:) 186:( 181:) 177:( 173:. 159:.

Index

secondary
tertiary
Lyall Watson
primatologists
Macaca fuscata
KĹŤjima
sweet potatoes
wheat
sand
observation
repetition
group consciousness
learned behavior
foreword
Lawrence Blair
Lifetide: The Biology of the Unconscious

cite
sources
improve this section
adding citations to reliable sources
removed
Learn how and when to remove this message
Japanese macaque
Ken Keyes Jr.
nuclear war
parable
secondary
tertiary
Primates

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.

↑