Knowledge (XXG)

Law of primacy in persuasion

Source 📝

184:
neutral to slightly positive experience for the rest of the days. The second group had a negative experience on the first day, and then a neutral to slightly positive experience for the rest of the stay. Groups three and four experienced a neutral experience for the first four days, with group three then having a positive experience on the last day and group four having a negative experience on the last day. Each group was then tested for customer satisfaction regarding their hypothetical stay. Garnefeld and Steinhoff found that the timing of positive or negative occurrences is what affected satisfaction. For negative events, a recency effect was demonstrated, meaning that negative events which occurred at the end of the stay affected customer satisfaction more than negative events at the beginning of the stay. For positive events, a primacy effect was demonstrated, which means that positive events that occurred at the beginning of the stay affected customer satisfaction more than positive events at the end of the stay. Therefore, the timing of particular types of events in extended service encounters predicts the effect of the event on satisfaction.
207:
election, some received a call three days before an election, and some residents did not receive a call. The call made was a nonpartisan attempt to mobilize people to vote. Results indicate that the timing of the call may not have an effect on the general voter population, but that the timing can affect certain populations and get certain populations to vote at higher rates. According to Panagopoulos, high-propensity voters, voters who typically turn out in higher numbers, voted at higher percentages when they received the call four weeks prior to the election, demonstrating a primacy effect. However, for lower propensity voters, voters who typically do not vote, calls made three days prior to the election were more effective at getting this population to vote, demonstrating a recency effect. Therefore, for voter mobilization, propensity to vote appears to be a moderating variable influencing the effect of timing of a mobilization call.
175:
video. One group made the assessments at the end of the video, one group made delayed assessments at the end of the video, a third group made the assessments after each skill in the video, and a fourth group that made the overall assessment after each skill but then made the assessment of the ability after viewing the entire video. Results indicate that a primacy effect was exhibited in each of the conditions, except for the third group, which did not demonstrate any order effects. The assessment of ability tended to agree with the initial ability level shown. However, the group that made assessments in an extended step-by-step manner were not influenced by the order the abilities were shown. Therefore, a primacy effect can occur in ability assessment, unless extended step-by-step processing is employed.
193:
information about sleep apnea treatments. The three groups were based on the order in which the information was presented. One group received information in an order unrelated to their values, while two groups received information ordered based on their values. One of the groups received information that aligned with their values first, and the other group received information that aligned with their values last. The researchers found that patients were more likely to choose the treatment that aligned with their values when this information was presented first, thus demonstrating a primacy effect for information on treatments that align with a patient's values. The order in which patients receive information appears to influence which treatment option they choose.
157:
questionnaire about the topics. For college students, the controversial topic exhibited a primacy effect. College students did not exhibit any order effects for a noncontroversial topic. The college students were persuaded more by the argument they encountered first for a controversial issue, but were not influenced by the order of the presentation of arguments for noncontroversial topics. However, for high school students, no order effects were exhibited for either the controversial or noncontroversial topics. Therefore, the controversy of a topic appears to affect the role order effects play for some age groups in persuasion.
122:
need for cognition, and were not experimentally manipulated into the two groups. Initially, both groups were equally persuaded by the message. However, the two groups then listened to a weak counter-message, which was not as strong as the evidence for the initial message. The group that had high need for cognition were not persuaded by the weak counter-message, and their opinions were still in line with the initial message. The other group, the low need for cognition group, was persuaded by the new message. The high need for cognition demonstrated a primacy effect, while the low need for cognition demonstrated a recency effect.
134:
for and against an exam policy, and this information was presented as being either chunked or unchunked. When the information was chunked, those who show high need for cognition were prone to primacy effects, while those with low need for cognition were prone to recency effects. However, when the information was presented in an unchunked nature, the opposite results were found. People who had high need for cognition demonstrated recency effects, whereas people who had low need for cognition showed a primacy effect. Therefore, chunking appears to interact with need for cognition to allow for order effects to occur.
86:
adjusted as new information is processed. This model predicts order effects based on the type of mental processing that is used for the new information. For end-of-sequence processing, or processing that occurs once all of the information has been presented, the model predicts primacy effects. The initial piece of information serves as the anchor, and subsequent pieces of information are aggregated together to adjust the initial piece of information. Therefore, the initial piece of information is weighted more than subsequent pieces of information, leading to a primacy effect.
143:
another group to gain little familiarization with the topic. A third group was then presented with no familiarization talk. Each of these three groups was then split into two subgroups, which listened to arguments for and against the topic twelve days later. One subgroup would listen to the argument for the topic first and then the argument against the topic, and the second subgroup would listen to the argument against it first. These groups then filled out a
114:
into their narrative of the crime. The side who presented the confession spoke first. Jurors who had high need for cognition were more likely to exhibit a primacy effect, meaning that they believed the confession fit with whichever side presented the confession as evidence. Conversely, jurors who had low need for cognition demonstrated a recency effect, and believed that the confession supported the case for the side who did not submit the confession.
133:
interacts with the order of information and need for cognition to moderate the occurrence of order effects. Petty, Tormala, Hawkins, and Wegener (2001) conducted a study which examined the effect chunking has on order effects in people with high and low need for cognition. Participants read arguments
49:
Schultz (1963) developed the "sensory-variation" hypothesis for order effects, which suggests that humans seek high activation and will respond to novel stimuli more strongly than to stimuli they are familiar with. Novel stimuli should provide higher activation than familiar information, according to
28:
in 1925 holds that the side of an issue presented first will have greater effectiveness in persuasion than the side presented subsequently. Lund presented college students with a document in support of one side of a controversial issue and then presented a second document which supported the opposite
113:
moderates the occurrence of order effects. Kassin, Reddy, and Tulloch (1990) demonstrated that a juror's need for cognition affects which order effect the juror relies on for their vote. An ambiguous confession was played by one side of the case, and then both sides commented that the confession fit
89:
However, when processing changes to step-by-step processing, or processing that occurs after each new piece of information, recency effects are predicted. Each new piece of information received will be processed separately. This new piece of information will then become the new anchor, which forms a
54:
When a person encounters a topic for the first time, the communication is perceived as novel and causes high activation. Any subsequent communication on the same topic produces less activation, and so the person will react according to the initial communication in an attempt to experience the higher
183:
In a study conducted by Garnefeld and Steinhoff (2013), order effects were demonstrated for opinions regarding service encounters. Four groups received daily descriptions of a hypothetical hotel stay over the course of five days. One group had a very positive experience on the first day, and then a
156:
A study conducted by Lana (1963) demonstrates that the controversiality of the topic can have a moderating effect on order effects. College students and high school students read arguments for and against a controversial topic and a noncontroversial topic, and were then asked to fill out an opinion
121:
This impact of need for cognition is supported by a study conducted by Huagtvedt and Petty (1992). The experimenters played a message for two groups of people, one group that was high in need for cognition and one group that was low in need for cognition. The two groups were determined by their own
117:
Kassin, Reddy, and Tulloch (1990) believed that the reason for this effect was due to the nature of processing that the jurors engaged in. Jurors who were high in need for cognition actively process the information. This active processing leads to agreeing with the initial presentation of the data,
147:
questionnaire on their opinions on the topic. The results show that prior familiarization with a topic increased the likelihood of a primacy effect. Therefore, those in the long familiarization group had an opinion on the topic that coincided with which argument they heard first, regardless of the
85:
Hogarth and Einhorn (1992) proposed the belief-adjustment model to try to predict in which situations order effects would occur and what specific order effect will occur. According to Hogarth and Einhorn, early information forms an initial impression, which is called an anchor. This anchor is then
192:
Order effects may be used to influence a patient to receive an effective treatment that aligns with their values. One study, conducted by Bansback, Li, Lynd, and Bryan (2014), demonstrates that a primacy effect will influence the decision for treatment. The researchers presented three groups with
62:
When a time delay or another task is inserted between the two communications, the higher level of activation will be dissipated by the time the second communication occurs. The second communication acts as a novel stimulus in this scenario. Either a recency effect or no order effect will occur in
76:
Anderson (1981) theorized that order effects occur due to "attention decrement". According to this theory, when the first piece of information is presented about an opinion, people tend to pay less attention to subsequent information that may provide evidence to the contrary opinion. Therefore,
174:
player performing certain skills. Two videos were shown, either in descending ability or increasing ability. The participants were then asked to make assessments of the overall ability of the players and three aspects of their ability. However, the assessments occurred at differing times in the
142:
A study conducted by Lana (1961) demonstrates a moderating effect of the familiarity of information presented on order effects. In the study, a topic of initial low familiarity was used. Then, the researchers presented a long talk to well familiarize one group with the topic and a short talk to
206:
In a study conducted by Panagopoulos (2010), order effects were found in terms of voter mobilization. Calls were made to residents of an American city at different times before an election. Some residents received a call 4 weeks prior to an election, some received a call two weeks prior to an
66:
When communications are familiar, such as social issues or current issues, the first communication will not produce activation that is less than the activation for unfamiliar communication. However, some aspect of the communication may be novel, and so there may be some increased activation.
58:
If the person is allowed to record his opinion after each communication, the person will respond more to the communication that he encounters either second in a group of two or last in a group of three or more in order to maintain a high level of activation. Therefore, a recency effect is
90:
new impression. Beliefs are being adjusted with the processing of each new anchor, which leads to more weight being placed to the information most recently received. Therefore, recency effects are predicted to occur when information is processed in a step-by-step manner.
77:
people's opinions about something are influenced strongly by the information that they paid attention to, which was the first information presented to them. This model of "attention decrement" predicts a primacy effect in opinion formation.
148:
actual stance. However, no prior familiarization led to a recency effect to be demonstrated. Therefore, the group that was given no familiarization talk demonstrated opinions that coincided with the argument presented last to them.
29:
position. He found the document read first had greater influence, regardless of which position it expressed. This empirical evidence was generally accepted until 1950, when Cromwell published findings of the opposite: a
118:
and then participating in processing that confirms this agreement. However, people low in need for cognition do not process information, and therefore, rely on the information presented most recently for their opinion.
33:
in which arguments presented later had greater effectiveness in persuasion than arguments presented first. It now appears that both primacy and recency effects occur in persuasion.
170:
Smith, Greenlees, and Manley (2009) found that order effects can occur in assessment of sports ability. The researchers had participants watch a video composed of an
601:
Smith, M. J.; Greenlees, I. & Manley, A. (2009). "Influence of order effects and mode of judgement on assessments of ability in sport".
263: 786: 297: 271: 409: 566:
Lana, Robert E. (1963). "Controversy of the topic and the order of presentation in persuasive communications".
41:
There are many different theoretical models proposed to explain the occurrence of primacy and recency effects.
712:
Panagopoulos, C. (2011). "Timing Is Everything? Primacy and Recency Effects in Voter Mobilization Campaigns".
460:"Personality and persuasion: Need for cognition moderates the persistence and resistance of attitude changes" 370:
Hogarth, R. M. & Einhorn, H. J. (1992). "Order effects in belief updating: The belief-adjustment model".
99: 644:
Garnefeld, I. & Steinhoff, L. (2013). "Primacy versus recency effects in extended service encounters".
293: 25: 216: 410:"Juror interpretations of ambiguous evidence: The need for cognition, presentation order, and persuasion" 130: 102:
the occurrence of order effects. Moderating factors affect the likelihood of order effects occurring.
781: 672: 737: 729: 626: 583: 513: 440: 432: 387: 334: 110: 759:
Lund, Frederick Hansen (1925). "The Psychology of Belief IV: The Law of Primacy in Persuasion".
694: 618: 548: 531:
Lana, R. E. (1961). "Familiarity and the order of presentation of persuasive communications".
20:
communication, the order of the information's presentation influences opinion formation. The
721: 684: 653: 610: 575: 540: 505: 474: 424: 379: 326: 245: 171: 268:
ADV 382J: Fall 2001, "Theories of Persuasive Communication & Consumer Decision Making"
249: 494:"Motivation to think and order effects in persuasion: The moderating role of chunking" 775: 741: 587: 493: 444: 383: 338: 517: 391: 630: 144: 67:
Therefore, either a small primacy effect or no order effect will be demonstrated.
478: 317:
Schultz, D. P. (1963). "Primacy-recency within a sensory variation framework".
725: 689: 657: 614: 579: 17: 509: 50:
this theory. Shultz developed four postulates from this general hypothesis:
698: 622: 552: 459: 236:
Stone, Vernon A. (1969). "A Primacy Effect in Decision-Making by Jurors".
733: 492:
Petty, R. E.; Tormala, Z. L.; Hawkins, C. & Wegener, D. T. (2001).
436: 428: 330: 544: 55:
activation. A primacy effect should be observed in this scenario.
673:"Exploiting order effects to improve the quality of decisions" 408:
Kassin, S. M.; Reddy, M. E. & Tulloch, W. F. (1990).
671:
Bansback, N.; Li L. C.; Lynd, L. & Bryan S. (2014).
24:, otherwise known as a primacy effect, as postulated by 294:"Order Effects Theory : Primacy versus Recency" 533:The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 354:Foundations of information integration theory 8: 467:Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 458:Haugtvedt, C. P. & Petty, R. E. (1992). 498:Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 296:. Center for Interactive Advertising, The 270:. Center for Interactive Advertising, The 761:Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 688: 228: 7: 403: 401: 365: 363: 161:Order effects in judgment formation 94:Factors that moderate order effects 250:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1969.tb00846.x 14: 677:Patient Education and Counseling 1: 646:Journal of Service Management 298:University of Texas at Austin 272:University of Texas at Austin 384:10.1016/0010-0285(92)90002-J 188:Order effects in health care 22:law of primacy in persuasion 356:. New York: Academic Press. 98:There are factors that can 81:The belief-adjustment model 803: 603:Journal of Sports Sciences 479:10.1037/0022-3514.63.2.308 138:Familiarity of information 726:10.1007/s11109-010-9125-x 690:10.1016/j.pec.2014.05.021 658:10.1108/09564231311304198 615:10.1080/02640410902939647 580:10.2466/pr0.1963.12.1.163 197:Order effects in politics 510:10.1177/0146167201273007 319:The Psychological Record 238:Journal of Communication 152:Controversy of the topic 352:Anderson, N.H. (1981). 417:Law and Human Behavior 217:Serial position effect 37:Theoretical background 568:Psychological Reports 26:Frederick Hansen Lund 787:Communication design 372:Cognitive Psychology 292:Kohler, Christine. 72:Attention decrement 714:Political Behavior 429:10.1007/BF01055788 331:10.1007/BF03393511 202:Voter mobilization 179:Service encounters 111:Need for cognition 106:Need for cognition 264:"Primacy-Recency" 45:Sensory-variation 794: 768: 746: 745: 709: 703: 702: 692: 668: 662: 661: 641: 635: 634: 598: 592: 591: 563: 557: 556: 545:10.1037/h0046634 528: 522: 521: 489: 483: 482: 464: 455: 449: 448: 414: 405: 396: 395: 367: 358: 357: 349: 343: 342: 314: 308: 307: 305: 304: 289: 283: 282: 280: 279: 260: 254: 253: 233: 172:ultimate Frisbee 802: 801: 797: 796: 795: 793: 792: 791: 772: 771: 758: 755: 753:Further reading 750: 749: 711: 710: 706: 670: 669: 665: 643: 642: 638: 600: 599: 595: 565: 564: 560: 530: 529: 525: 491: 490: 486: 462: 457: 456: 452: 412: 407: 406: 399: 369: 368: 361: 351: 350: 346: 316: 315: 311: 302: 300: 291: 290: 286: 277: 275: 262: 261: 257: 235: 234: 230: 225: 213: 204: 199: 190: 181: 168: 163: 154: 140: 128: 108: 96: 83: 74: 47: 39: 12: 11: 5: 800: 798: 790: 789: 784: 774: 773: 770: 769: 754: 751: 748: 747: 704: 683:(2): 197–203. 663: 636: 609:(7): 745–752. 593: 574:(1): 163–170. 558: 539:(3): 573–577. 523: 504:(3): 332–344. 484: 450: 397: 359: 344: 325:(2): 129–139. 309: 284: 255: 244:(3): 239–247. 227: 226: 224: 221: 220: 219: 212: 209: 203: 200: 198: 195: 189: 186: 180: 177: 167: 164: 162: 159: 153: 150: 139: 136: 127: 124: 107: 104: 95: 92: 82: 79: 73: 70: 69: 68: 64: 63:this scenario. 60: 56: 46: 43: 38: 35: 31:recency effect 13: 10: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 799: 788: 785: 783: 780: 779: 777: 766: 762: 757: 756: 752: 743: 739: 735: 731: 727: 723: 719: 715: 708: 705: 700: 696: 691: 686: 682: 678: 674: 667: 664: 659: 655: 651: 647: 640: 637: 632: 628: 624: 620: 616: 612: 608: 604: 597: 594: 589: 585: 581: 577: 573: 569: 562: 559: 554: 550: 546: 542: 538: 534: 527: 524: 519: 515: 511: 507: 503: 499: 495: 488: 485: 480: 476: 472: 468: 461: 454: 451: 446: 442: 438: 434: 430: 426: 422: 418: 411: 404: 402: 398: 393: 389: 385: 381: 377: 373: 366: 364: 360: 355: 348: 345: 340: 336: 332: 328: 324: 320: 313: 310: 299: 295: 288: 285: 273: 269: 265: 259: 256: 251: 247: 243: 239: 232: 229: 222: 218: 215: 214: 210: 208: 201: 196: 194: 187: 185: 178: 176: 173: 165: 160: 158: 151: 149: 146: 137: 135: 132: 125: 123: 119: 115: 112: 105: 103: 101: 93: 91: 87: 80: 78: 71: 65: 61: 59:hypothesized. 57: 53: 52: 51: 44: 42: 36: 34: 32: 27: 23: 19: 764: 760: 720:(1): 79–93. 717: 713: 707: 680: 676: 666: 652:(1): 64–81. 649: 645: 639: 606: 602: 596: 571: 567: 561: 536: 532: 526: 501: 497: 487: 470: 466: 453: 423:(1): 43–55. 420: 416: 375: 371: 353: 347: 322: 318: 312: 301:. Retrieved 287: 276:. Retrieved 267: 258: 241: 237: 231: 205: 191: 182: 169: 155: 145:Likert scale 141: 129: 120: 116: 109: 97: 88: 84: 75: 48: 40: 30: 21: 15: 782:Advertising 378:(1): 1–55. 776:Categories 473:(2): 308. 303:2007-11-04 278:2007-11-04 223:References 18:persuasive 767:: 183–91. 742:143912398 588:144779674 445:144360961 339:148818862 734:41488275 699:24961445 623:19437187 553:14461892 518:38839119 392:54272129 211:See also 131:Chunking 126:Chunking 100:moderate 631:9619845 437:1393555 740:  732:  697:  629:  621:  586:  551:  516:  443:  435:  390:  337:  274:. 2001 166:Sports 738:S2CID 730:JSTOR 627:S2CID 584:S2CID 514:S2CID 463:(PDF) 441:S2CID 433:JSTOR 413:(PDF) 388:S2CID 335:S2CID 695:PMID 619:PMID 549:PMID 722:doi 685:doi 654:doi 611:doi 576:doi 541:doi 506:doi 475:doi 425:doi 380:doi 327:doi 246:doi 16:In 778:: 765:20 763:. 736:. 728:. 718:33 716:. 693:. 681:96 679:. 675:. 650:24 648:. 625:. 617:. 607:27 605:. 582:. 572:12 570:. 547:. 537:62 535:. 512:. 502:27 500:. 496:. 471:63 469:. 465:. 439:. 431:. 421:14 419:. 415:. 400:^ 386:. 376:24 374:. 362:^ 333:. 323:13 321:. 266:. 242:19 240:. 744:. 724:: 701:. 687:: 660:. 656:: 633:. 613:: 590:. 578:: 555:. 543:: 520:. 508:: 481:. 477:: 447:. 427:: 394:. 382:: 341:. 329:: 306:. 281:. 252:. 248::

Index

persuasive
Frederick Hansen Lund
moderate
Need for cognition
Chunking
Likert scale
ultimate Frisbee
Serial position effect
doi
10.1111/j.1460-2466.1969.tb00846.x
"Primacy-Recency"
University of Texas at Austin
"Order Effects Theory : Primacy versus Recency"
University of Texas at Austin
doi
10.1007/BF03393511
S2CID
148818862


doi
10.1016/0010-0285(92)90002-J
S2CID
54272129


"Juror interpretations of ambiguous evidence: The need for cognition, presentation order, and persuasion"
doi
10.1007/BF01055788
JSTOR

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.