Knowledge (XXG)

Usage-based models of language

Source 📝

630:. Langacker identified commonly recurring linguistic patterns (patterns such as those associated with Wh- fronting, subject-verb agreement, the use of present participles, etc.) and represented these supposed rule-governed behaviours on a hierarchical structure. The Cognitive Grammar model represented grammar, semantics and lexicon as associated processes that were laid on a continuum, which provided a theoretical framework that was significant in studying the usage-based conception of language. Consequently, a usage-based model accounts for these rule-governed language behaviours by providing a representational scheme that is entirely instance-based, and able to recognize and uniquely represent each familiar pattern, which occurs with varying strengths at different instances. His usage-based model draws on the cognitive psychology of schemata, which are flexible hierarchical structures that are able to accommodate the complexity of mental stimuli. Similarly, as humans perceive linguistic abstractions as multilayered, ranging from patterns that occur across whole utterances to those that occur in phonetic material, the usage-based model acknowledges the differing levels of granularity in speakers’ knowledge of their language. Langacker's work emphasizes that both abstract structure and instance-based detail are contained in language, differing in granularity but not in basic principles. 641:’s work greatly inspired the creation of usage-based models of language. Bybee’s model makes predictions about and explains synchronic, diachronic and typological patterns within languages, such as which variants will occur in which contexts, what forms they will take, and about their diachronic consequences. Using the linguistic phenomenon of splits (when a word starts to show subtle polysemy, and morphological possibilities for the originally single form ensue), Bybee proves that even irreducibly irregular word-forms are seen to be non-arbitrary when the context it occurs in is taken into consideration in the very representation of morphology. Simultaneously, she shows that even seemingly regular allomorphy is context-sensitive. Splits also aligns with the idea that linguistic forms cannot be studied as isolated entities, but rather in relation to the strength of their attachment to other entities. 604:(1987) applied these concepts to linguistic studies. For usage-based models of language, these discoveries legitimized interest in the peripheral phenomena and inspired the examination of the ontological status of the rules themselves. Secondly, WCCF focuses on the effects of social/ textual context and cognitive processes on human thought, instead of established systems and representations, which motivated the study of external sources in usage-based language research. For example, in analyzing the differences between the grammatical notions of subject vs. topic, Li and Thompson (1976), found that the repetition of certain topics by a 652:’s "Entrenchment-and-Conventionalization" Model offers a comprehensive recent summary approach to usage-based thinking. In great detail and with reference to many sub-disciplines and concepts in linguistics he shows how usage mediates between entrenchment, the establishment of linguistic habits in individuals via repetition and associations, and conventionalization, a continuous feedback cycle which builds shared collective linguistic knowledge. All three components connect linguistic utterance types with their respective situative settings and extralinguistic associations. 738:
representation. And when stored representations are accessed, the representations change. Additionally, memory storage can store detailed information about processed tokens during linguistic experience, including form and context that these tokens were used. In this model, general categories and grammar units can emerge from linguistic experiences stored in memories, as exemplars are categorized by similarity to each other. Contiguous experiences such as meaning and acoustic shape are also recorded to be linked to each other.
751:
transitions between elements within a chunk. Additionally, the individual elements of a chunk can link to elements in other contexts. The example of ‘drive someone crazy’ forms a chunk, however items that compose it are not analyzable individually as words that occur elsewhere in cognitive representation. As chunks are used more frequently, words can lose their associations with exemplars of the same word. This is known as
448: 513:), on considering language as an isolated system removed from its use in human interaction and human cognition. Rather, usage-based models posit that linguistic information is expressed via context-sensitive mental processing and mental representations, which have the cognitive ability to succinctly account for the complexity of actual language use at all levels ( 579:. It studies the lifespan of linguistic units (e.g. words, suffixes), arguing that they can survive language change through frequent usage or by participating in usage-based generalizations if their syntactic, semantic or pragmatic features overlap with other similar constructions. There is disagreement as to whether the approach is different from 746:
By these means repeated sequences become more fluent. Within a chunk, sequential links are graded in strength based on the frequency of the chunk or perhaps the transitions between the elements of a chunk. A construction is a chunk even though it may contain schematic slots, that is, the elements of
713:
perspective, constructions are groupings of words with idiosyncratic behaviour to a certain extent. They mostly take on an unpredictable meaning or pragmatic effect, or are formally special. From a broader perspective, construction can also be seen as processing units or chunks, such as sequences of
750:
Memory storage requires links to connect idiomatic phrases together. In chunking, repeated sequences are represented together as units which can be accessed directly. Through this, repeated sequences are more frequent. Sequential links are assessed in strength based on the frequency of the chunk or
705:
Constructions have direct pairing of form to meaning without intermediate structures, making them appropriate for usage-based models. The usage-based model adopts constructions as the basic unit of form-meaning correspondence. A construction is commonly regarded to be a conventionalized string of
737:
Adjectives shown here include crazy, mad, and up the wall, which are semantically related to the word drive. In exemplar models, the idea that memory for linguistic experience is similar to memory for other types of memories is proposed. Every token of linguistic experience impacts cognitive
608:
resulted in the surfacing and crystallization of formal properties into syntactic entities, namely the subject. This notion of syntax and morphology being an outcome of pragmatic and cognitive factors was influential in the development of usage-based models. Thirdly, the WCCF methodology of
613:
is similarly practised in usage-based models, in collecting data from real communicative contexts and analyzing them for typological regularities. This highlights an important aspect of usage-based research, the study of methods for the integration of synchrony and diachrony.
722:
or form. Additionally, chunks or conventionalized sequences can tend to develop special pragmatic implications that can lead to special meaning over time. They can also develop idiosyncrasies of form in a variety of ways.
504:
framework, that emerged since the late 1980s, and that assumes a profound relation between linguistic structure and usage. It challenges the dominant focus, in 20th century linguistics (and in particular in
680:
argues that the frequency explanation is circular—certain patterns are often used by people because they are frequent—and that the explanation of frequency issues must be found outside themselves.
548:
in 1987. Usage-based models of language have become a significant new trend in linguistics since the early 2000s. Influential proponents of usage-based linguistics include
996: 867: 1107:
Advances in Corpus Linguistics: Papers from the 23rd International Conference on English Language Research on Computerized Corpora (ICAME 23) Göteborg 22-26 May 2002
1245: 718:) which have been used often enough to be accessed together. This implicates that common words sequences are sometimes constructions even if they do not have 672:
which more frequently only has a direct object in actual language usage ('sell something'). It is hypothesized that such differences in the recurrence of the
1531: 596:
West Coast cognitive functionalism (WCCF) played a major role in the creation of the usage-based enterprise. Firstly, a crucial point in WCCF was
600:’s paper on semantic categories in human cognition, which studied fuzzy semantic categories with central and peripheral concepts. Subsequently, 537:
and processing, synchronic and diachronic patterns, and both low-level and high-level structure in language, by looking at actual language use.
1221: 825: 475: 1285: 1055:
Hopper, P. J. (1997). "When 'Grammar' and Discourse Clash: The Problem of Source Conflicts". In J. Bybee; J. Haiman; S. A. Thompson (eds.).
706:
words. A key feature of a grammar based on constructions is that it can reflect the deeply intertwined lexical items and grammar structure.
365: 1510: 1390: 1268: 660:
Advocates of usage-based linguistics including Joan Bybee and Martin Haspelmath argue that statistics of language usage depend on
325: 103: 385: 360: 51: 1195:
Luce, P.A.; Pisoni, D.B; Goldinger, S.D. (1990). "Similarity neighborhoods of spoken words". In G. T. M. Altmann (ed.).
808:
von Mengden, Ferdinand; Coussé, Evie (2014). "Introduction. The role of change in usage-based conceptions of language".
305: 171: 425: 131: 468: 415: 315: 141: 774: 576: 320: 263: 78: 1040:
Givon, T (1989). "Modes of knowledge and modes of processing. The routinization of behavior and information".
1109:. Language and Computers: Studies in Practical Linguistics. Vol. 49. Amsterdam: Rodopi. pp. 85–100. 966:
Boyes-Braem, P; Johnson, D; Gray, W.; Mervis, C.B.; Rosch, E. (1976). "Basic objects in natural categories".
1105:
Murkherjee, J. (2004). "Corpus Data in a Usage-Based Cognitive Grammar". In K. Aijmer; B. Altenberg (eds.).
572: 420: 258: 235: 1373:
Goldberg, Adele E.; Casenhiser, Devin (2006). "English Constructions". In Bas Aarts; April McMahon (eds.).
557: 497: 370: 337: 290: 206: 186: 166: 68: 46: 41: 861: 501: 146: 769: 752: 700: 564: 534: 461: 390: 300: 181: 126: 23: 1010:
Givon, T (1979b). "From discourse to syntax: Grammar as a processing strategy". In T. Givón (ed.).
779: 764: 677: 649: 610: 231: 161: 136: 108: 447: 1483: 1423: 1239: 1087: 990: 506: 451: 430: 400: 355: 310: 278: 268: 156: 151: 1406:
Bybee, Joan L.; Eddington, David (2006). "A usage-based approach to Spanish verbs of becoming".
882: 1506: 1386: 1264: 1227: 1217: 905: 821: 627: 549: 295: 273: 216: 1070:
Greenberg, J.H. (1960). "A quantitative approach to the morphological typology of language".
1473: 1465: 1415: 1378: 1308: 1079: 942: 897: 813: 784: 623: 605: 568: 545: 395: 226: 221: 196: 191: 176: 676:
depend on statistical learning based on the language usage encountered by the individual.
673: 1213:
The dynamics of the linguistic system : usage, conventionalization, and entrenchment
719: 240: 1197:
Cognitive Models of Speech Processing: Psycholinguistic and Computational Perspectives
1525: 1427: 1091: 597: 1487: 1122:
Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding: an inquiry into human knowledge structures
1182:
The evolution of grammar: tense, aspect, and modality in the languages of the world
601: 510: 283: 73: 812:. Studies in Functional and Structural Linguistics. Vol. 69. pp. 1–20. 493: 435: 410: 31: 1382: 1469: 1231: 946: 901: 638: 553: 526: 405: 88: 842: 715: 661: 530: 518: 514: 380: 375: 211: 201: 93: 83: 1313: 1211: 909: 1478: 1419: 1305:
Proceedings of the 14th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society
817: 1330:
Radical Construction Grammar: Syntactic Theory in Typological Perspective
843:"From a Usage-Based Model to Usage-Based Instruction: Testing the theory" 580: 710: 668:
always has two arguments ('tell something to someone') unlike the verb
1303:
Filmore, Charles J. (1988). "The mechanisms of Construction Grammar".
522: 98: 1360:
Constructions: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure
1083: 930: 1286:"Usage-based Theory and Exemplar Representations of Constructions" 533:). Broadly speaking, a usage-based model of language accounts for 1345:
Constructions at Work: The Nature of Generalizations in Language
1137:
Morphology: A study of the relation between meaning and form
575:, usage-based linguistics belongs to the wider framework of 1014:. Vol. 12. New York: Academic Press. pp. 81–109. 644: 850:
International Journal of Education and Human Developments
730:
The death of his wife the following year drove him mad.
1443:
Frequency and the Emergence of Linguistic Structure
841:Serafima Gettys, Patricia Bayona, Rocío Rodríguez. 664:. For instance, it is argued that the English verb 645:
Schmid's Entrenchment-and-Conventionalization model
1180:Bybee, J. L.; Perkins, R.D.; Pagliuca, W. (1994). 981:Iverson, G.K., Lima, S.D.; Corrigan, R.L. (1994). 1167:Frequency of use and the organization of language 1377:. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 343–55. 1059:. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. pp. 231–247. 881:Christiansen, Morten H.; Chater, Nick (2008). 1199:. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. pp. 122–147. 1072:International Journal of American Linguistics 1057:Essays on Language Function and Language Type 1042:Mind, Code, and Context: Essays in Pragmatics 803: 801: 799: 469: 8: 1456:Ellis, Nick C. (1996). "Sequencing in SLA". 1154:. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. 995:: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list ( 866:: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list ( 1044:. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. pp. 237–268. 1244:: CS1 maint: location missing publisher ( 563:Together with related approaches, such as 476: 462: 18: 1477: 1312: 810:Usage-Based Approaches to Language Change 1441:Bybee, Joan L.; Hopper, Paul J. (2001). 1362:. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 1184:. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 795: 30: 1458:Studies in Second Language Acquisition 1237: 988: 859: 634:Bybee's Dynamic Usage-based framework 622:The term ‘usage-based’ was coined by 7: 1169:. New York: Oxford University Press. 1120:Schank, R.C.; Abelson, R.P. (1977). 684:Constructions: Form-meaning pairings 366:Conservative and innovative language 1375:The Handbook of English Linguistics 1532:Linguistic theories and hypotheses 1347:. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 1332:. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 592:West coast cognitive functionalism 14: 883:"Language as shaped by the brain" 626:in 1987, while doing research on 1307:. Vol. 14. pp. 35–55. 931:"Memes shape brains shape memes" 733:This room drives me up the wall. 446: 983:The reality of linguistic rules 1263:. Cambridge University Press. 1: 1503:Unified Theories of Cognition 935:Behavioral and Brain Sciences 890:Behavioral and Brain Sciences 618:Langacker’s Cognitive Grammar 16:Linguistics approach / theory 1445:. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 1139:. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 985:. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 747:a chunk can be interrupted. 306:Functional discourse grammar 172:Ethnography of communication 1358:Goldberg, Adele E. (1995). 1343:Goldberg, Adele E. (2006). 1029:. New York: Academic Press. 426:Second-language acquisition 1548: 1383:10.1002/9780470753002.ch15 1216:(First ed.). Oxford. 1210:Schmid, Hans-Jörg (2020). 1152:Phonology and language use 698: 496:approach within a broader 104:Syntax–semantics interface 1470:10.1017/S0272263100014698 1124:. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 947:10.1017/S0140525X08005037 929:Blackmore, Susan (2008). 902:10.1017/S0140525X08004998 583:or essentially the same. 416:Philosophy of linguistics 316:Interactional linguistics 1505:. Cambridge: MIT Press. 1027:On understanding grammar 775:Evolutionary linguistics 577:evolutionary linguistics 1328:Croft, William (2001). 1290:Oxford Handbooks Online 1259:Song, Jae Jung (2012). 742:Constructions as chunks 573:complex adaptive system 490:usage-based linguistics 1501:Newell, Allen (1990). 1314:10.3765/bls.v14i0.1794 253:Theoretical frameworks 207:Philosophy of language 187:History of linguistics 1420:10.1353/lan.2006.0081 1165:Bybee, J. L. (2006). 1150:Bybee, J. L. (2001). 1135:Bybee, J. L. (1985). 818:10.1075/sfsl.69.01men 656:Frequency explanation 147:Conversation analysis 1012:Discourse and Syntax 968:Cognitive Psychology 770:Construction grammar 753:de-categorialization 701:Construction grammar 571:, and language as a 565:construction grammar 535:language acquisition 391:Internet linguistics 301:Construction grammar 1025:Givon, T. (1979c). 780:Linguistic typology 765:Applied linguistics 727:It drives me crazy. 611:linguistic typology 558:Morten Christiansen 326:Systemic functional 121:Applied linguistics 63:General linguistics 587:Disciplinary roots 431:Theory of language 401:Origin of language 356:Autonomy of syntax 311:Grammaticalization 157:Discourse analysis 152:Corpus linguistics 1223:978-0-19-254637-1 827:978-90-272-1579-6 628:Cognitive Grammar 550:Michael Tomasello 521:, morphology and 486: 485: 274:Distributionalism 217:Psycholinguistics 1539: 1517: 1516: 1498: 1492: 1491: 1481: 1453: 1447: 1446: 1438: 1432: 1431: 1403: 1397: 1396: 1370: 1364: 1363: 1355: 1349: 1348: 1340: 1334: 1333: 1325: 1319: 1318: 1316: 1300: 1294: 1293: 1281: 1275: 1274: 1256: 1250: 1249: 1243: 1235: 1207: 1201: 1200: 1192: 1186: 1185: 1177: 1171: 1170: 1162: 1156: 1155: 1147: 1141: 1140: 1132: 1126: 1125: 1117: 1111: 1110: 1102: 1096: 1095: 1067: 1061: 1060: 1052: 1046: 1045: 1037: 1031: 1030: 1022: 1016: 1015: 1007: 1001: 1000: 994: 986: 978: 972: 971: 963: 957: 956: 954: 953: 926: 920: 919: 917: 916: 887: 878: 872: 871: 865: 857: 847: 838: 832: 831: 805: 785:Ronald Langacker 696: 695: 691: 650:Hans-Jörg Schmid 624:Ronald Langacker 606:speech community 569:emergent grammar 546:Ronald Langacker 478: 471: 464: 450: 396:LGBT linguistics 386:Internationalism 361:Compositionality 222:Sociolinguistics 197:Neurolinguistics 192:Interlinguistics 177:Ethnomethodology 19: 1547: 1546: 1542: 1541: 1540: 1538: 1537: 1536: 1522: 1521: 1520: 1513: 1500: 1499: 1495: 1455: 1454: 1450: 1440: 1439: 1435: 1405: 1404: 1400: 1393: 1372: 1371: 1367: 1357: 1356: 1352: 1342: 1341: 1337: 1327: 1326: 1322: 1302: 1301: 1297: 1284:Bybee, Joan L. 1283: 1282: 1278: 1271: 1258: 1257: 1253: 1236: 1224: 1209: 1208: 1204: 1194: 1193: 1189: 1179: 1178: 1174: 1164: 1163: 1159: 1149: 1148: 1144: 1134: 1133: 1129: 1119: 1118: 1114: 1104: 1103: 1099: 1069: 1068: 1064: 1054: 1053: 1049: 1039: 1038: 1034: 1024: 1023: 1019: 1009: 1008: 1004: 987: 980: 979: 975: 965: 964: 960: 951: 949: 928: 927: 923: 914: 912: 885: 880: 879: 875: 858: 845: 840: 839: 835: 828: 807: 806: 797: 793: 761: 703: 697: 693: 689: 687: 686: 674:indirect object 658: 647: 589: 482: 441: 440: 351: 343: 342: 254: 246: 245: 241:Writing systems 132:Anthropological 122: 114: 113: 64: 56: 17: 12: 11: 5: 1545: 1543: 1535: 1534: 1524: 1523: 1519: 1518: 1511: 1493: 1479:2027.42/139863 1448: 1433: 1398: 1391: 1365: 1350: 1335: 1320: 1295: 1276: 1269: 1251: 1222: 1202: 1187: 1172: 1157: 1142: 1127: 1112: 1097: 1084:10.1086/464575 1078:(3): 178–194. 1062: 1047: 1032: 1017: 1002: 973: 958: 921: 896:(5): 489–558. 873: 833: 826: 794: 792: 789: 788: 787: 782: 777: 772: 767: 760: 757: 735: 734: 731: 728: 720:idiosyncrasies 699:Main article: 685: 682: 657: 654: 646: 643: 588: 585: 544:was coined by 484: 483: 481: 480: 473: 466: 458: 455: 454: 443: 442: 439: 438: 433: 428: 423: 421:Prescriptivism 418: 413: 408: 403: 398: 393: 388: 383: 378: 373: 368: 363: 358: 352: 349: 348: 345: 344: 341: 340: 335: 334: 333: 328: 323: 318: 313: 308: 303: 298: 288: 287: 286: 281: 276: 271: 266: 255: 252: 251: 248: 247: 244: 243: 238: 229: 224: 219: 214: 209: 204: 199: 194: 189: 184: 179: 174: 169: 164: 159: 154: 149: 144: 139: 134: 129: 123: 120: 119: 116: 115: 112: 111: 106: 101: 96: 91: 86: 81: 76: 71: 65: 62: 61: 58: 57: 55: 54: 49: 44: 38: 35: 34: 28: 27: 15: 13: 10: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 1544: 1533: 1530: 1529: 1527: 1514: 1512:9780674921016 1508: 1504: 1497: 1494: 1489: 1485: 1480: 1475: 1471: 1467: 1463: 1459: 1452: 1449: 1444: 1437: 1434: 1429: 1425: 1421: 1417: 1414:(2): 323–55. 1413: 1409: 1402: 1399: 1394: 1392:9780470753002 1388: 1384: 1380: 1376: 1369: 1366: 1361: 1354: 1351: 1346: 1339: 1336: 1331: 1324: 1321: 1315: 1310: 1306: 1299: 1296: 1291: 1287: 1280: 1277: 1272: 1270:9780521872140 1266: 1262: 1255: 1252: 1247: 1241: 1233: 1229: 1225: 1219: 1215: 1214: 1206: 1203: 1198: 1191: 1188: 1183: 1176: 1173: 1168: 1161: 1158: 1153: 1146: 1143: 1138: 1131: 1128: 1123: 1116: 1113: 1108: 1101: 1098: 1093: 1089: 1085: 1081: 1077: 1073: 1066: 1063: 1058: 1051: 1048: 1043: 1036: 1033: 1028: 1021: 1018: 1013: 1006: 1003: 998: 992: 984: 977: 974: 969: 962: 959: 948: 944: 940: 936: 932: 925: 922: 911: 907: 903: 899: 895: 891: 884: 877: 874: 869: 863: 855: 851: 844: 837: 834: 829: 823: 819: 815: 811: 804: 802: 800: 796: 790: 786: 783: 781: 778: 776: 773: 771: 768: 766: 763: 762: 758: 756: 754: 748: 744: 743: 739: 732: 729: 726: 725: 724: 721: 717: 712: 707: 702: 692: 683: 681: 679: 678:Jae Jung Song 675: 671: 667: 663: 655: 653: 651: 642: 640: 636: 635: 631: 629: 625: 620: 619: 615: 612: 607: 603: 599: 598:Eleanor Rosch 594: 593: 586: 584: 582: 578: 574: 570: 566: 561: 559: 555: 551: 547: 543: 538: 536: 532: 528: 524: 520: 516: 512: 508: 503: 499: 495: 491: 479: 474: 472: 467: 465: 460: 459: 457: 456: 453: 449: 445: 444: 437: 434: 432: 429: 427: 424: 422: 419: 417: 414: 412: 409: 407: 404: 402: 399: 397: 394: 392: 389: 387: 384: 382: 379: 377: 374: 372: 371:Descriptivism 369: 367: 364: 362: 359: 357: 354: 353: 347: 346: 339: 338:Structuralism 336: 332: 329: 327: 324: 322: 321:Prague circle 319: 317: 314: 312: 309: 307: 304: 302: 299: 297: 294: 293: 292: 289: 285: 282: 280: 277: 275: 272: 270: 267: 265: 262: 261: 260: 257: 256: 250: 249: 242: 239: 237: 233: 230: 228: 225: 223: 220: 218: 215: 213: 210: 208: 205: 203: 200: 198: 195: 193: 190: 188: 185: 183: 180: 178: 175: 173: 170: 168: 167:Documentation 165: 163: 160: 158: 155: 153: 150: 148: 145: 143: 142:Computational 140: 138: 135: 133: 130: 128: 125: 124: 118: 117: 110: 107: 105: 102: 100: 97: 95: 92: 90: 87: 85: 82: 80: 77: 75: 72: 70: 67: 66: 60: 59: 53: 50: 48: 45: 43: 40: 39: 37: 36: 33: 29: 25: 21: 20: 1502: 1496: 1461: 1457: 1451: 1442: 1436: 1411: 1407: 1401: 1374: 1368: 1359: 1353: 1344: 1338: 1329: 1323: 1304: 1298: 1289: 1279: 1260: 1254: 1212: 1205: 1196: 1190: 1181: 1175: 1166: 1160: 1151: 1145: 1136: 1130: 1121: 1115: 1106: 1100: 1075: 1071: 1065: 1056: 1050: 1041: 1035: 1026: 1020: 1011: 1005: 982: 976: 967: 961: 950:. Retrieved 938: 934: 924: 913:. Retrieved 893: 889: 876: 862:cite journal 853: 849: 836: 809: 749: 745: 741: 740: 736: 708: 704: 669: 665: 659: 648: 637: 633: 632: 621: 617: 616: 602:Robin Lakoff 595: 591: 590: 562: 541: 539: 511:generativism 489: 487: 330: 284:Glossematics 264:Constituency 236:interpreting 74:Lexicography 542:usage-based 494:linguistics 436:Terminology 411:Orthography 331:Usage-based 232:Translating 127:Acquisition 32:Linguistics 1464:: 91–126. 1261:Word Order 1232:1139239358 952:2020-12-22 941:(5): 513. 915:2020-12-22 791:References 714:words (or 711:grammarian 554:Joan Bybee 527:pragmatics 498:functional 406:Orismology 291:Functional 279:Generative 269:Dependency 89:Pragmatics 79:Morphology 69:Diachronic 1428:145635167 1240:cite book 1092:144261944 991:cite book 716:morphemes 662:frequency 540:The term 531:semantics 519:phonology 515:phonetics 507:formalism 502:cognitive 381:Iconicity 376:Etymology 296:Cognitive 259:Formalist 212:Phonetics 202:Philology 94:Semantics 84:Phonology 1526:Category 1488:17820745 1408:Language 910:18826669 759:See also 581:memetics 182:Forensic 162:Distance 109:Typology 24:a series 22:Part of 709:From a 137:Applied 47:History 42:Outline 1509:  1486:  1426:  1389:  1267:  1230:  1220:  1090:  908:  824:  688:": --> 523:syntax 452:Portal 350:Topics 99:Syntax 1484:S2CID 1424:S2CID 1088:S2CID 886:(PDF) 856:: 50. 846:(PDF) 670:sell, 639:Bybee 492:is a 52:Index 1507:ISBN 1387:ISBN 1265:ISBN 1246:link 1228:OCLC 1218:ISBN 997:link 906:PMID 868:link 822:ISBN 690:edit 666:tell 556:and 529:and 517:and 488:The 234:and 227:Text 1474:hdl 1466:doi 1416:doi 1379:doi 1309:doi 1080:doi 943:doi 898:doi 814:doi 1528:: 1482:. 1472:. 1462:18 1460:. 1422:. 1412:82 1410:. 1385:. 1288:. 1242:}} 1238:{{ 1226:. 1086:. 1076:26 1074:. 993:}} 989:{{ 939:31 937:. 933:. 904:. 894:31 892:. 888:. 864:}} 860:{{ 852:. 848:. 820:. 798:^ 755:. 567:, 560:. 552:, 525:, 26:on 1515:. 1490:. 1476:: 1468:: 1430:. 1418:: 1395:. 1381:: 1317:. 1311:: 1292:. 1273:. 1248:) 1234:. 1094:. 1082:: 999:) 970:. 955:. 945:: 918:. 900:: 870:) 854:4 830:. 816:: 694:] 509:- 500:/ 477:e 470:t 463:v

Index

a series
Linguistics
Outline
History
Index
Diachronic
Lexicography
Morphology
Phonology
Pragmatics
Semantics
Syntax
Syntax–semantics interface
Typology
Acquisition
Anthropological
Applied
Computational
Conversation analysis
Corpus linguistics
Discourse analysis
Distance
Documentation
Ethnography of communication
Ethnomethodology
Forensic
History of linguistics
Interlinguistics
Neurolinguistics
Philology

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.