Knowledge (XXG)

Media adequacy

Source 📝

178:
chosen because it connects cognitive styles with media effects. The former group will probably benefit significantly from discussion forums. For introspective users, on the other hand, they would more likely be a hindrance in the process of a successful information transfer. Another example: it has also long been known that some - not all - learners learn best by observing. Obviously, mirror neurons become active when they see corresponding efforts by other learners, so that their self-motivation and ultimately their learning success increases. Here it can be assumed that examples from the media (such as television programmes) can also have a positive effect. Conversely, other learners must have their own experiences in order to store and process information successfully. Mediated communication therefore means a limitation for them; on the other hand, it could be that users with individual concepts for learning have an advantage through and in online learning environments - at least compared to the 'observant learner type'.
61:, which is seen by many more viewers. The main reason is that television reports are only effective if they can be illustrated; this was hardly possible in the case of television reports about an illegal wiretapping. In the newspaper, on the other hand, it was possible to describe how the information from an (anonymous, i.e. not visually known) source was verified. The medium and its production constraints thus (partly) decide whether and how content can be presented or how effective the transfer of information is. 182:
tendency to avoid uncertainty - i.e. who have only a low willingness to take risks - tend to feel uncomfortable with self-learning media. There are also parallels to another observation described above: Since online media increase the degree of ambiguity both in terms of tasks and expectations of the respective user, users with a lower tolerance of uncertainty avoidance are disadvantaged, while users with a higher degree of uncertainty avoidance can even generate further advantages.
193:, the cultural imprint of the media user, etc.). Summarising, it is desirable to know whether and how a media-supported information transfer can be established sensibly and efficiently. However, it is questionable whether a system can be developed that links content, user type and medium, since the mentioned variables and their interactions already give rise to too many different situations, not least since technical developments are constantly changing the media and their effects. 158:
communication). This almost inevitably leads to a certain attention deficit. The use of weak media therefore means that utilising such media requires a greater cognitive effort on the part of the user. Perhaps this is why greater self-discipline is required in the context of media-supported learning than in traditional learning situations.
181:
Furthermore, different cognitive styles require a different density and volume of information in order to ultimately benefit from information transfer efforts in the context of meta cognitive processes. Perhaps this explains the findings of Baruch and Nicholson, who observed that learners with a high
161:
Relatively new, however, is the attempt to link media theory statements with studies on cognitive styles., Usually it is referred to traditional teaching, and it of course has to be taken into consideration that there are differences between information processing and learning (Rehder/Hoffmann 2005).
177:
All in all, it seems to be clear that a successful information transfer depends on the situation and the strategies of how content is mediatised. With some users, it might be successful in the context of interactive group processes, whilst others need quietness to concentrate. This example has been
254:
Sternberg, R J, & Grigorenko, E L (1997). Are cognitive styles still in style? American Psychologist, 52, 700-712.; Grigorenko, E L, & Sternberg, R J (1995). Thinking styles. In Saklogsk E, & D H, Zeidner, M (Eds.). International handbook of personality and intelligence (pp. 205-229).
136:
or reception or learner research, the scientific study of media adequacy is relatively new. It is only since the triumph of microelectronics that not only specialists have been able to publish with their medium. Meanwhile, in many areas there is the possibility (and opportunity, but often also the
73:
and it is investigated what influences (promotes or hinders) a successful transfer of information: Only what can be realised sensibly and efficiently in a particular medium should also be realised in that medium. If the researcher's perspective is not based on the recipient or the content, but on
185:
In assessing whether the use is meaningful at all, the observation that a media-supported information transfer is fundamentally more time-intensive than traditional learning also seems significant. Accordingly, it is important to use media-supported learning materials in such a way that the added
157:
and Robert Lengel. Daft and Lengel speak of different degrees of "media richness" depending on how much the media is absorbed: Less "rich" media are not intensive enough to attract a user's full and undisturbed attention (for example: discussion forums, chats, e-mails or other forms of textual
28:
The successful transfer of information depends on various aspects. An important aspect is of course the content itself. Depending on the goal, purpose and methods, the question of whether and why an information transfer is successful can depend on a more or less successful content preparation.
264:
Rehder B, & Hoffman, A B (2005). Eye tracking and selective attention in category learning. Cognitive Psychology, 51, 1-41.; Pillay, H (1998). An investigation of the effect of individual cognitive preferences on learning through computer-based instruction. Educational Psychology, 18,
173:
Sternberg's "theory of mental self-government" is quite complex and therefore sometimes difficult to operationalise. It assumes that under ideal (and free) conditions learners organise the learning process in accordance with their cognitive abilities. This leads to different forms of mental
89:. In the case of the Watergate Affair, for example, the transfer of information with the help of television was only successful in the form of secondary reporting. This did not mean, however, that visual communication is fundamentally impossible with a topic like this. The genre of the 41:
etc. It is however often underestimated that the channel via which information is transferred can also be of decisive importance. Question then is which medium makes it possible to convey a specific content as well as possible (and which medium is more of a hindrance).
29:
Further decisive factors can lie in the reception and the user's specific situation in its context (which is examined in recipient research, learner research, etc.), as well as in the social situation, which in turn depends on a variety of aspects, such as
226:
Daft, R L & Lengel, R H (1984). Information Richness: A new approach to managerial behavior and organizational design. In Cummings, L L & Staw, B M (Eds.). Research in organizational behavior. (6, pp. 191-233). Homewood, IL: JAI
162:
The common factor, however, is how one takes in and processes information and might even be able to adopt to new developments, to solve problems and reaches decisions. A pioneering theoretical approach to this is, for example, the "
292:
Hazeltine, E, Ruthruff, E, & Remington, R W (2006). The role of input and output modality pairings in dual-task performance: Evidence for content-dependent central interference. Cognitive Psychology, 52,
19:
refers to specific (i.e. media) aspects that are important for a successful transfer of information. This implies that not all information can be reproduced in an equally adequate way with every medium.
121:. Thus, the medium is as important as other variables, for example the learner type (social learner type, introspective learner type). Ultimately, it is a question of complex interactions between 105:) and not only to primary media. In addition to the constraints of production, the conditions of reception also play a role. Content on a website has a different effect when it is received on a 208:
Workman, M (2014). Performance in Computer-based and Computer-aided Education: Do Cognitive Styles Make a Difference? In: Journal of Computers in Human Behavior, Vol. 20, 2014, 517–534.
347:
Giessen, H W (2015). Media-Based Learning Methodology: Stories, Games, and Emotions. In: Ally, M & Khan, B H, The International Handbook of E-Learning. London: Routledge, 43–54.
311:
Giessen, H W (2015). Media-Based Learning Methodology: Stories, Games, and Emotions. In: Ally, M & Khan, B H, The International Handbook of E-Learning. London: Routledge, 43–54.
189:
There are other variables whose effects for a successful information transfer by now have been little or not at all investigated, at least not from a media perspective (such as
329:
Hayes, J, & Allinson, C W (1998). Cognitive style and the theory and practice of individual and collective learning in organizations. Human Relations, 51, 847-871.
274:
Hayes, J, & Allinson, C W (1998). Cognitive style and the theory and practice of individual and collective learning in organizations. Human Relations, 51, 847-871.
74:
the medium, media adequacy is the central category. Only what can be realised sensibly and efficiently in a particular medium should also be realised in that medium.
338:
Baruch, Y, & Nicholson, N (1997). Home, sweet work: Requirements for effective home-working. Journal of General Management, 23(2), 15-30.
137:
compulsion) to work cross-medially, so that the question of the appropriate (media-adequate) presentation of content becomes urgent.
145:
There are various theories about the use of media in different contexts and regarding different objectives. The approaches of
367: 45:
In general, it can be said that not all content can be transmitted equally well with every medium. One example is the
236:
Mason, R (2004). Online education using learning objects. British Journal of Educational Technology. 35, 752-754.
94: 245:
Flavell, J H (1992). Cognitive development: Past, present, and future. Developmental Psychology, 28, 998-1005.
146: 217:
McLuhan, H M (1962). The Gutenberg Galaxy: The making of typographic man. Toronto: University of Toronto Press
163: 150: 78: 57:
that is read by relatively few (though certainly influential and opinion-forming) people - but not by
362: 167: 320:
Bandura, A (1978). The self-system in reciprocal determinism. American Psychologist. 33, 344-358.
186:
value in terms of content is correspondingly high and the majority of learners benefit from it.
133: 122: 46: 106: 283:
Garner, H (2006). Five Minds for the Future. Boston: Harvard Business School Publishing
174:
representations or different codings, and thus to different forms of how to use media.
38: 356: 82: 154: 90: 50: 126: 102: 58: 302:
Sternberg, R J (1997). Thinking styles. New York: Cambridge University Press.
70: 54: 114: 110: 34: 190: 30: 117:
is more successful compared to working with the same content on a
98: 86: 118: 97:"). The question of media adequacy thus relates more to 8: 113:. Using one and the same content via the 201: 49:, which after all led to the fall of a 69:As a rule, the respective research is 7: 93:was very successful and effective (" 166:" that relates considerations from 81:” refers not only to the means of 14: 101:(in the field of print media: 53:. It was uncovered by a daily 1: 141:History, theory, observations 132:In contrast to, for example, 149:are well known, as is the " 384: 147:Herbert Marshall McLuhan 85:, but also to different 95:All the President's Men 368:Mass media technology 164:cognitive load theory 151:media richness theory 255:London: Plenum Press 168:Robert J. Sternberg 170:to media effects. 375: 348: 345: 339: 336: 330: 327: 321: 318: 312: 309: 303: 300: 294: 290: 284: 281: 275: 272: 266: 262: 256: 252: 246: 243: 237: 234: 228: 224: 218: 215: 209: 206: 134:content analysis 109:or a stationary 47:Watergate Affair 383: 382: 378: 377: 376: 374: 373: 372: 353: 352: 351: 346: 342: 337: 333: 328: 324: 319: 315: 310: 306: 301: 297: 291: 287: 282: 278: 273: 269: 263: 259: 253: 249: 244: 240: 235: 231: 225: 221: 216: 212: 207: 203: 199: 143: 67: 65:Research topics 26: 12: 11: 5: 381: 379: 371: 370: 365: 355: 354: 350: 349: 340: 331: 322: 313: 304: 295: 285: 276: 267: 257: 247: 238: 229: 219: 210: 200: 198: 195: 142: 139: 66: 63: 39:learning style 25: 22: 17:Media adequacy 13: 10: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 380: 369: 366: 364: 361: 360: 358: 344: 341: 335: 332: 326: 323: 317: 314: 308: 305: 299: 296: 289: 286: 280: 277: 271: 268: 261: 258: 251: 248: 242: 239: 233: 230: 223: 220: 214: 211: 205: 202: 196: 194: 192: 187: 183: 179: 175: 171: 169: 165: 159: 156: 152: 148: 140: 138: 135: 130: 128: 125:, medium and 124: 120: 116: 112: 108: 104: 100: 96: 92: 88: 84: 83:communication 80: 75: 72: 64: 62: 60: 56: 52: 48: 43: 40: 36: 32: 23: 21: 18: 343: 334: 325: 316: 307: 298: 288: 279: 270: 260: 250: 241: 232: 222: 213: 204: 188: 184: 180: 176: 172: 160: 155:Richard Daft 144: 131: 91:feature film 76: 68: 51:US president 44: 27: 16: 15: 363:Mass media 357:Categories 197:References 103:text types 77:The term “ 59:television 24:Definition 129:process. 127:reception 71:normative 55:newspaper 293:291-345. 265:171-182. 115:Internet 111:computer 123:content 35:culture 191:gender 107:tablet 99:genres 87:genres 79:medium 31:gender 227:Press 153:" by 119:DVD 359:: 37:, 33:,

Index

gender
culture
learning style
Watergate Affair
US president
newspaper
television
normative
medium
communication
genres
feature film
All the President's Men
genres
text types
tablet
computer
Internet
DVD
content
reception
content analysis
Herbert Marshall McLuhan
media richness theory
Richard Daft
cognitive load theory
Robert J. Sternberg
gender
Categories
Mass media

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.