Knowledge (XXG)

Oblique effect

Source 📝

107:
oblique effect may be due to selectivity for cardinal (i.e. horizontal and vertical) orientations in higher level visual areas and more specifically in parahippocampal place area (PPA), an area devoted to scene perception. This finding is supported by the fact that, among all visual object categories, perception of scenes (both natural and man-made environments) receives more processing benefit from the oblique effect and higher visual acuity for horizontal and vertical contours, due to their unique structure.
124:
inevitable consequence of the way neural signals are processed, or whether it is a minor error that nature hadn't been bothered to correct, or whether it fulfills a function in making us better in handling our visual environment. Proposing a "purpose" of the oblique effect, and developing scientific support for it is still a work in progress. A popular concept is that we live in a
49: 123:
Nevertheless, there is an oblique effect for target configurations that do not directly address these "oriented" neural elements early in the visual path into the brain. Regardless of where in the brain of the human or animals an oblique effect is found, one would still like to know whether it is an
99:
Neural processing of contours was highlighted by the classical research by Hubel and Wiesel which revealed neural units right at the entrance of visual signals into the brain that respond preferentially to lines and edges. When the distribution of preferred orientation of these units was examined,
106:
However, in contrast to the strong behavioral effect, evidence for orientation selectivity bias in primary visual cortex is weak and controversial. Actually, many human fMRI studies have failed to see this biased activity in primary visual cortex. Rather, more recent studies have suggested that,
59:
The effect is exhibited predominantly in tasks involving discrimination of the angle of tilt of patterns or contours. People are very good at detecting whether a picture is hung vertical, but are two- to fourfold worse for a 45-degree oblique contour, even when a comparison is available. However
131:
Competing explanations have to contend with questions, not yet finalized, of innateness of horizontal/vertical superiority, of body symmetry in anatomical organization, of methodology of measurement, and particularly, of issues associated with perceptual development in infants and children, and
31:
completed an experiment in which he set a line to make it appear parallel to an adjoining one, and found observers' errors to be least for horizontal and vertical orientations and largest for an inclination of 45 degrees. The effect can be demonstrated for many visual tasks and was named
90:
one looks at the neural apparatus. Much pertinent information has been gathered here, yet the phenomenon was discovered in, and has ultimate relevance to, the whole organism's performance. Hence it is not contradictory to follow two separate tracks of explanation.
116: 181:
the overestimation of vertical distances in vision, is not generally encompassed by the oblique effect, which mostly lumps the vertical and horizontal together in making comparisons with the obliques.
60:
there is no oblique deficit in some other tasks, such as judgment of lengths. Similarly, while it is harder to judge the direction of motion when it is oblique, this is not the case for speed.
329:
Essock, Edward A.; DeFord, J. Kevin; Hansen, Bruce C.; Sinai, Michael J. (June 2003). "Oblique stimuli are seen best (not worst!) in naturalistic broad-band stimuli: a horizontal effect".
155:
In vision, a plane containing the anterior-posterior axis of the eye. According to standards in the eye professions, the left side of the horizontal meridian, as seen by the subject, has
63:
The figure on the right shows the performance when an observer makes judgments about the length (top) and the orientation (bottom) of a line, in eight orientations around the clock.
170:
When presented a natural or other broad-band scene, people see oblique content the best and they actually see horizontal content the worst, with vertical usually falling in between
128:
environment. Attempts at empirical explanations of perceptual visual phenomena have led to the examination of the orientation distribution of contours in the everyday visual world.
70:, changes on a 45-degree rotation—the geometrical congruity of the square and the diamond does not extend to their perception as figures (see left) as was emphasized by 19:
is the name given to the relative deficiency in perceptual performance for oblique contours as compared to the performance for horizontal or vertical contours.
310:
Westheimer, G. (2003). Meridional anisotropy in visual processing: implications for the neural site of the oblique effect. Vision Research, 43 (22), 2281-2289.
274:
Freeman J, Brouwer GJ, Heeger DJ, Merriam EP (2011) Orientation decoding depends on maps, not columns. Journal of Neuroscience 31(13):4792-4804
319:
Howe CQ, Purves D. (2005) Perceiving geometry : geometrical illusions explained by natural scene statistics Springer : New York
223:
Hubel, D.H., & Wiesel, T.N. (1959). Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat's striate cortex. J. Physiol., 148, 574-591.
245:
Li, W., & Gilbert, C.D. (2002). Global Contour Saliency and Local Colinear Interactions . J Neurophysiol, 88 (5), 2846-2856.
283:
Nasr S, Tootell RBH (2012) A cardinal orientation bias in scene-selective visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 32(43):14921-6
103:
Orientation differences also occur in testing the visual brain with probes for cell connectivity and with imaging techniques.
83: 176: 141: 199:
Sitzungsberichte der Mathematisch-Naturwissenschaftlichen Classe der Kaiserlichen Akademie der Wissenschaften, Wien
292:
Epstein RA, Kanwisher N (1998) A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature 392:598–601
119:
Discrimination of length (top) and orientation (bottom) for a line at various orientations around the clock
383: 67: 354: 346: 338: 246: 378: 232:
Li B, Peterson MR, Freeman RD, (2003) Oblique effect: a neural basis in the visual cortex
125: 37: 342: 372: 301:
Torralba A, Oliva A (2003) Statistics of natural image categories. Network 14:391-412
388: 210:
Appelle S. 1972 Perception and discrimination as function of stimulus orientation.
197:
Mach, E. 1861 Ueber das Sehen von Lagen und Winkeln durch die Bewegung des Auges.
159:, and orientations increase in a clockwise direction, again as seen by subject. 261:
Furmanski CS, Engel SA (2000) An oblique effect in human primary visual cortex
100:
there were fewer in the oblique meridians than in the vertical and horizontal.
71: 28: 350: 358: 115: 168:
The horizontal effect is an extension of the oblique effect in which...
48: 27:
The earliest known observation of this effect came about in 1861 when
250: 66:
Even the immediate appearance of the form of a figure, often called
114: 47: 86:
the oblique effect can be examined at two levels. The
8: 165:directions are horizontal and vertical. 190: 55:of figure changes on 45-degree rotation 7: 14: 36:in the widely cited article by 1: 343:10.1016/s0042-6989(03)00142-1 84:geometrical-optical illusions 177:Vertical-horizontal illusion 142:Judgment of Line Orientation 78:Origin of the oblique effect 405: 234:Journal of Neurophysiology 212:Psychological Bulletin 120: 56: 118: 51: 263:Nature Neuroscience 121: 57: 337:(12): 1329–1335. 157:0-deg orientation 132:across cultures. 396: 363: 362: 326: 320: 317: 311: 308: 302: 299: 293: 290: 284: 281: 275: 272: 266: 259: 253: 251:10.1152/jn.00289 243: 237: 230: 224: 221: 215: 208: 202: 195: 404: 403: 399: 398: 397: 395: 394: 393: 369: 368: 367: 366: 331:Vision Research 328: 327: 323: 318: 314: 309: 305: 300: 296: 291: 287: 282: 278: 273: 269: 260: 256: 244: 240: 231: 227: 222: 218: 209: 205: 196: 192: 187: 150: 138: 113: 97: 80: 46: 25: 12: 11: 5: 402: 400: 392: 391: 386: 381: 371: 370: 365: 364: 321: 312: 303: 294: 285: 276: 267: 254: 238: 225: 216: 203: 201:43(2), 215-224 189: 188: 186: 183: 149: 146: 145: 144: 137: 134: 112: 109: 96: 93: 79: 76: 45: 44:The phenomenon 42: 38:Stuart Appelle 34:oblique effect 24: 21: 17:Oblique effect 13: 10: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 401: 390: 387: 385: 382: 380: 377: 376: 374: 360: 356: 352: 348: 344: 340: 336: 332: 325: 322: 316: 313: 307: 304: 298: 295: 289: 286: 280: 277: 271: 268: 264: 258: 255: 252: 248: 242: 239: 235: 229: 226: 220: 217: 213: 207: 204: 200: 194: 191: 184: 182: 180: 178: 173: 171: 166: 164: 160: 158: 154: 147: 143: 140: 139: 135: 133: 129: 127: 117: 110: 108: 104: 101: 95:Physiological 94: 92: 89: 88:physiological 85: 77: 75: 73: 69: 64: 61: 54: 50: 43: 41: 39: 35: 30: 22: 20: 18: 334: 330: 324: 315: 306: 297: 288: 279: 270: 262: 257: 241: 233: 228: 219: 211: 206: 198: 193: 175: 174: 169: 167: 162: 161: 156: 152: 151: 130: 122: 105: 102: 98: 87: 81: 65: 62: 58: 52: 33: 26: 16: 15: 236:90, 204-217 126:carpentered 384:Perception 373:Categories 214:78,266-278 185:References 72:Ernst Mach 53:Appearance 29:Ernst Mach 23:Background 351:0042-6989 265:3,535-536 153:Meridian: 111:Empirical 359:12742103 163:Cardinal 136:See also 82:As with 68:gestalt 379:Vision 357:  349:  148:Notes 355:PMID 347:ISSN 389:Eye 339:doi 247:doi 375:: 353:. 345:. 335:43 333:. 172:. 74:. 40:. 361:. 341:: 249:: 179:,

Index

Ernst Mach
Stuart Appelle

gestalt
Ernst Mach
geometrical-optical illusions

carpentered
Judgment of Line Orientation
Vertical-horizontal illusion
doi
10.1152/jn.00289
doi
10.1016/s0042-6989(03)00142-1
ISSN
0042-6989
PMID
12742103
Categories
Vision
Perception
Eye

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.