Knowledge (XXG)

Compound subject

Source 📝

336:
Choose one (the traditional rule for most languages has been to treat mixed groups as male; some modern rules, on the other hand, call for randomly choosing one sex or the other, alternating choosing male and female in successive similar circumstances, or even for simply choosing the female gender in
308:
the verb only for stylistic reasons; or even as a mandatory requirement, in languages with V-S-O or V-O-S word order and a strongly fixed word order. These languages often use different strategies for handling subjects after vs. before the verb: for example, tending to prefer an "agree with the
329:
Closest agreement: Simply agree with the constituent noun phrase that is closest to the verb, and ignore the rest. This generally applies only to subject-verb agreement; pronominal agreement is by its nature long-distance, and so the concept of "closest" makes less sense in this
31:
between the subject and other entities (verbs, pronouns, etc.). These issues also occur with compound noun phrases of all sorts, but the problems are most acute with compound subjects because of the large number of types of agreement occurring with such subjects.
312:
Multiple strategies have been used to handle both compound subjects in general and category disagreement between/among the coordinate members of the subject. Languages often differ in which strategies they use. Among the strategies are:
88:
are coordinated. The tendency, in such cases, is to rewrite the sentences to avoid the conjunction: e.g. "Sylvia and I each have our own car, and one of us is planning to sell their car". This still has a compound subject using
317:
General agreement: Consider the total set of entities involved, work out their properties, and assign a pronoun accordingly. (E.g. if the set of entities includes two women referred to in the third person, and the language has
340:
Use a construction (e.g. "he or she", "his or her", "our(s)") that captures both or all values of the disagreeing property. Often this is considered awkward and to be avoided.
241:. In many European languages, for example, a standard problem occurs with mixed-gender compound subjects. This problem does not appear in English, because 309:
closest phrase" strategy with a following subject, for pragmatic reasons, even when an "agree with the whole" strategy is used in other circumstances.
412:
Children are often taught in school as a matter of politeness that they should always order oneself after any others in a compound subject and use
70:
generally take a plural verb. However, there are exceptions. When compound subjects are thought of as a single unit, a singular verb is used, e.g.
97:" to get around the "his or her" problem. This could be avoided with a further rewrite: "Either Sylvia will sell her car, or I will sell mine." 393:
for an argument of a verb when occurring directly before a finite verb. On the other hand, in compound subjects in informal speech,
351:
An additional concern in English is that there are special rules for pronouns in compound subjects. Although English has
556: 322:, as in Arabic, then the group's properties would be "dual number, female, third person", with corresponding pronoun 333:
If using "general agreement" and there is a disagreement among properties (e.g. some male, some female), either:
319: 284:
In addition, some languages allow subjects to follow verbs: either optionally for stylistic reasons, as in
541: 274: 24: 43:, the agreement rules are generally unambiguous, but sometimes tricky. For example, the compound subject 465: 55:
car", not "your car", "their car", etc.). In languages with more extensive subject-verb agreement (e.g.
28: 526: 278: 27:
to form a single, longer noun phrase. Compound subjects cause many difficulties in compliance with
85: 81: 186:
i.e. either one of them will sell that one's own car, or one of them will sell someone else car
516: 273:
has gender as well as person and number agreement on its verbs, and more specifically in its
226:
i.e. one of us will sell the car he or she owns, assuming that I am male and Sylvia is female
536: 531: 521: 365: 297: 293: 258: 238: 56: 36: 285: 270: 246: 60: 301: 296:("Now are entering John, Jim, and their wives"); as the normal state of affairs, as in 289: 369:
is not a living characteristic of the spoken language, and hence the case-based terms
550: 166:
ambiguous between whether we own two cars jointly, or each owns one car individually
495: 21: 80:, the rules are often ill-defined, especially when two elements that differ in 63:), the verb agreement is clearly revealed as also being first-person plural. 237:
Additional concerns appear in compound subjects in languages other than
218:. Remove the difficulty by changing the auxiliary verb to just "will".) 436:, when adopted by speakers it typically is generalized to compound 206:
i.e. one of us will sell the car he owns, assuming that I am male
281:, there is gender agreement with third-person plural subjects. 489:
Halliday, M.A.K & Matthiessen, Christian M. I. M (2004).
482:
Everaert, M.; van Riemsdijk, H.; Goedemans, R. (eds) 2006.
401:(possibly because of the lack of direct agreement between 141:"Either my sisters are coming to visit, or my mother is.") 76:
As shown in the examples, if the subjects are joined by
385:) are misleading. In general, in the spoken language, 176:
i.e. they jointly own the car, one of them will sell it
93:
as the conjunction, and uses "semi-informal" "generic
72:
Peanut butter and jelly is available in the cafeteria.
216:
i.e. we jointly own the car, one of us will sell it
196:
i.e. we jointly own the car, together we'll sell it
188:: if the "someone else" has just been mentioned.) 158:are going to sell your (you guys', your own) car. 432:. Although intended specifically for compound 224:are/am/is? going to sell their(??) own car. ( 8: 504:A student's introduction to English grammar. 51:, taking appropriate pronominal agreement (" 457: 145:My mother or father is coming to visit. 496:An introduction to functional grammar 7: 355:case distinctions in pronouns (e.g. 277:and in the more conservative spoken 502:Huddleston, R.; Pullum, K. (2005). 214:are/am/is? going to sell our car. ( 20:consists of two or more individual 440:, leading to alternations such as 14: 499:. Hodder Arnold, London, England. 486:, Volumes I–V, Blackwell, London. 484:The Blackwell companion to syntax 399:Johnny and me are coming tomorrow 204:are going to sell his? own car. ( 397:occurs in this position, e.g. 347:Additional concerns in English 1: 424:and in a compound object use 174:is going to sell their car. ( 164:are going to sell our cars. ( 194:are going to sell our car. ( 152:are going to sell their car. 66:Compound subjects joined by 39:compound subjects joined by 506:Cambridge University Press. 184:is going to sell his car. ( 47:is treated equivalently to 573: 389:is the default form, but 245:is genderless. But e.g. 137:is/are coming to visit. ( 261:similarly has masculine 135:My sisters or my mother 542:Subject verb agreement 491:Subject, actor, theme 127:is coming to visit. ( 29:grammatical agreement 527:Subjective (grammar) 121:are coming to visit. 466:"Compound Subjects" 292:or occasionally in 557:Syntactic entities 442:between me and you 86:grammatical number 82:grammatical gender 517:Subject (grammar) 446:between you and I 304:, where subjects 275:literary language 115:went to the park. 109:went up the hill. 564: 537:English pronouns 532:Grammatical case 522:Object (grammar) 470: 469: 462: 366:grammatical case 320:grammatical dual 298:Classical Arabic 269:. In addition, 233:General concerns 18:compound subject 572: 571: 567: 566: 565: 563: 562: 561: 547: 546: 513: 479: 474: 473: 464: 463: 459: 454: 371:subjective case 349: 235: 103: 12: 11: 5: 570: 568: 560: 559: 549: 548: 545: 544: 539: 534: 529: 524: 519: 512: 509: 508: 507: 500: 487: 478: 475: 472: 471: 456: 455: 453: 450: 379:objective case 348: 345: 344: 343: 342: 341: 338: 331: 327: 249:has masculine 234: 231: 230: 229: 219: 209: 199: 189: 179: 169: 159: 153: 147: 142: 132: 122: 116: 110: 102: 99: 13: 10: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 569: 558: 555: 554: 552: 543: 540: 538: 535: 533: 530: 528: 525: 523: 520: 518: 515: 514: 510: 505: 501: 498: 497: 492: 488: 485: 481: 480: 476: 467: 461: 458: 451: 449: 447: 443: 439: 435: 431: 430:me and Johnny 427: 426:Johnny and me 423: 419: 415: 410: 408: 404: 400: 396: 392: 388: 384: 380: 376: 372: 368: 367: 362: 358: 354: 353:morphological 346: 339: 335: 334: 332: 328: 325: 321: 316: 315: 314: 310: 307: 303: 299: 295: 291: 287: 282: 280: 276: 272: 268: 265:vs. feminine 264: 260: 256: 253:vs. feminine 252: 248: 244: 240: 232: 227: 223: 220: 217: 213: 210: 207: 203: 200: 197: 193: 190: 187: 183: 180: 177: 173: 170: 167: 163: 160: 157: 154: 151: 148: 146: 143: 140: 136: 133: 130: 126: 123: 120: 119:John and Jill 117: 114: 111: 108: 107:Jack and Jill 105: 104: 100: 98: 96: 92: 87: 83: 79: 74: 73: 69: 64: 62: 58: 54: 50: 46: 42: 38: 33: 30: 26: 23: 19: 503: 494: 490: 483: 477:Bibliography 460: 445: 441: 437: 433: 429: 425: 422:I and Johnny 421: 418:Johnny and I 417: 413: 411: 406: 402: 398: 394: 390: 386: 382: 378: 374: 370: 364: 360: 356: 352: 350: 323: 311: 305: 283: 266: 262: 254: 250: 242: 236: 225: 221: 215: 211: 205: 201: 195: 191: 185: 181: 175: 171: 165: 161: 156:You and John 155: 149: 144: 138: 134: 128: 125:John or Jill 124: 118: 112: 106: 94: 90: 77: 75: 71: 67: 65: 52: 48: 44: 40: 34: 22:noun phrases 17: 15: 428:instead of 420:instead of 337:all cases). 222:Sylvia or I 212:Sylvia or I 182:John or Jim 172:John or Jim 129:Better with 113:Haley and I 25:coordinated 452:References 381:(e.g. for 373:(e.g. for 202:John and I 192:John and I 162:John and I 150:John and I 139:Rewrite as 416:, saying 279:varieties 131:"either") 45:you and I 551:Category 511:See also 434:subjects 101:Examples 438:objects 306:precede 294:English 259:Spanish 239:English 57:Spanish 37:English 377:) vs. 324:humatā 286:German 271:Arabic 247:French 61:Arabic 330:case. 302:Irish 290:Latin 267:ellas 263:ellos 255:elles 95:their 444:vs. 405:and 359:vs. 300:and 243:they 35:For 493:in 409:). 407:are 363:), 288:, 251:ils 91:and 84:or 68:and 59:or 53:our 41:and 553:: 448:. 403:me 395:me 387:me 383:me 361:me 326:.) 257:; 78:or 49:we 16:A 468:. 414:I 391:I 375:I 357:I 228:) 208:) 198:) 178:) 168:)

Index

noun phrases
coordinated
grammatical agreement
English
Spanish
Arabic
grammatical gender
grammatical number
English
French
Spanish
Arabic
literary language
varieties
German
Latin
English
Classical Arabic
Irish
grammatical dual
grammatical case
"Compound Subjects"
An introduction to functional grammar
Subject (grammar)
Object (grammar)
Subjective (grammar)
Grammatical case
English pronouns
Subject verb agreement
Category

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.