Knowledge

Encoding specificity principle

Source 📝

74:, a pioneer of research into memory, noted that associations between items aids recall of information thus the internal context of a list matters. This is because we look for any connection that helps us combine items into meaningful units. This started a lot of research into lists of to-be-remembered (tbr) words, and cues that helped them. In 1968 Tulving and Osler made participants memorise a list of 24 tbr words in the absence or presence of cue words. The cue words facilitated recall when present in the input and output of memorising and recalling the words. They concluded that specific retrieval cues can aid recall if the information of their relation to the tbr words is stored at the same time as the words on the list. Tulving and Thomson studied the effect of the change in context of the tbr by adding, deleting and replacing context words. This resulted in a reduction in the level of recognition performance when the context changed, even though the available information remained context. This led to the encoding specificity principle. 287:(AD) are unable to effectively process the semantic relationship between two words at encoding to assist in the retrieval process. The general population benefits equally from a weakly related cue word as from a strongly related cue word during a recall task, provided the weakly related word was present at encoding. Patients with AD, however, were unable to benefit from the weakly related cue even if it was present at both encoding and retrieval. Instead of relying upon semantic encoding, those with AD presented their most dominant associations to the cue words during recall test. This explains why all AD patients performed well when two strong words were matched together but very poorly when a strong and weak pairs were presented during recall. Deficits in episodic memory are now widely accepted as a characteristic symptom of Alzheimer's disease. 270:
that when presented with Russian-language cues, participants recalled memories that occurred in a Russian-speaking environment and when presented with English-language cues, they easily recalled memories from English-speaking contexts. This is first because the cue words may have been spoken during the original event that the participant was remembering; hearing the word at encoding and again at retrieval may have been a sufficient cue to bring the memory to mind. Second, this phenomenon may be due to the general language-created ambiance of the situation in which participants were tested rather than the specific associations to individual cue words.
258:
recall and recognition during a test. In the study 39 participants were asked to read through an article one time, knowing that they would take a short test on the material. Each of the participants wore headphones while reading but some of the participants heard moderately loud background noise and others heard nothing. They found that regardless of the type of test, it is more beneficial to study and test in the same auditory environment. In line with the encoding specificity principle, this mismatch at encoding and retrieval is detrimental to test performance.
342:
suggests that what determines successful memory is cue distinctiveness. He says that good memory may be produced even if there is almost no encoding-retrieval overlap, provided the minimal overlap is highly distinctive. He characterizes memory as an "active process of discrimination" and proposes that we use cues to choose between several retrieval candidates. Increasing the encoding-retrieval match improves memory performance, he believes, but only because it increases the probability that distinctive features will come into play.
333:
on application of principles to new examples, then one should practice by applying principles during the study session. When students know the requirements for a test or the performance task they can better encode the information while studying and can perform at a higher level when tested. Studying information in a manner that is closest to the method of assessment is the optimal method of studying due to it aiding recall of the information in a similar context to that of the assessment.
319:(trying to carefully remember the visual content of the commercial) led to a much higher rate of recall for emotional advertisements. Conversely, al peptions, preferences of given object advertised) led to a much higher recall of specific advertisements. Empirical evidence regarding the nature of emotional advertising provides the advertising industry with data as to how to contour their ads to maximize recall of advertisements. 299:, is retrieved more effectively when an individual is intoxicated as compared to being sober. State-dependent memory is one example of encoding specificity. If an individual encodes information while intoxicated he or she, ideally, should match that state when attempting to recall the encoded information. This type of state-dependent effect is strongest with free recall rather than when strong retrieval cues are present. 303:
intoxication. This principle demonstrates the significance of encoding specificity; the contextual state of intoxication provides retrieval cues and information that are superior to and outweigh the negative effects on memory from a depressant substance that activates GABA and inhibits neurotransmission. In this regard, this encoding specific context trumps the importance of such neural brain activity.
47:
pattern; the specific encoding pattern may vary from instance to instance, even if nominally the item is the same, as encoding depends on the context. This conclusion was drawn from a recognition-memory task. A series of psychological experiments were undertaken in the 1970s which continued this work and further showed that context affects our ability to recall information.
346:
positively, not due to them being encoded at the time of learning but due to pre-experimentally derived associations. Suggesting that the word on the list 'came to mind' at the time of the experiment and that anyone could have given the positive answer. This is seen as even more likely with strong cues. This is known as the 'lucky guessing' criticism.
241:
while underwater in scuba gear, the other was given the same list on dry land. When asked to recall the information the participants remembered the list of words better when tested in the environment where the list was studied. This experiment illustrates how recreating the physical environment of encoding can aid in the retrieval process.
249:: context can be a useful cue for memory but only when it is needed. One will only turn to context as a cue when better cues are unavailable. In recognition tests, cues other than the immediate encoding context and environment are superior, whereas in free-recall tests, the immediate environment serves as the only cue to trigger memory. 269:
are more accessible when the language at encoding and recall match. Researchers conducted interviews with Russian and English speaking bilingual students in both languages and asked participants to retrieve the first memory that comes to mind when hearing a generic word in either language. They found
257:
The level and kind of noise in any given encoding environment will affect the ability to recall the information encoded in a different auditory environment. Grant, et al. (1998) performed a study to test how the auditory environment during encoding and the auditory environment during testing effected
85:
do not always play a role in encoding specificity; memory, rather, depends upon the context at encoding and retrieval. Early research has shown that semantically related cues should be effective in retrieving a word provided the semantic cue was encoded along with the target word. If the semantically
332:
The encoding specificity principle has an implication for studying; as the recall of information is aided by the context of encoding the information, suggesting one should study in a similar context to the exam. The way an individual studies should match the way he or she is tested. If one is tested
353:
conducted experiments on the encoding specificity principle to check the generalisability of the concept. The first experiment focused on the normative strength go the cues presented on the encoding and recall of words and the second on the presence of weak cues in seconding and recall. The results
240:
The location and environment in which one learns something readily affects how freely it is recalled. In an experiment by Godden and Baddeley in 1975, researchers took two groups of individuals and asked them to study and remember a list of given words. One group was given a list of words to study
93:
Regardless of semantic relatedness of the paired words, participants more effectively recalled target words that had been primed when prompted for recall. Many of the following experiments employed a method modeled off of Thomson and Tulving's. All, however, had slight variations which allowed the
89:
In a laboratory study, a subject presented with an unrelated word pair is able to recall a target word with much more accuracy when prompted with the unrelated word it was matched with at the time of encoding, than if presented with a semantically related word that was not available during the time
323:
displays this emotional nature of content. A political advertisement from Lyndon B. Johnson's 1964 presidential campaign is inherently emotional in nature and therefore very easily remembered. If this advertisement re viewed and encoded in an episodic mode, due to its emotional nature, it would be
46:
who suggested that contextual information is encoded with memories which affect the retrieval process. When a person uses information stored in their memory it is necessary that the information is accessible. The accessibility is governed by retrieval cues, these cues are dependent on the encoding
345:
Phillip Higham has also criticised the design and interpretation of Thomson and Tulving's original experiments which used strong and weak cues to generate the encoding specificity principle. He states that the use of forced-report retrieval may have resulted in participants responding to the cues
244:
The type of environment itself did not matter, just that the environment was constant during encoding and recall, as the effect on recall of the environment of recall depends on the environment of original learning. Memory tested through recognition, however, was not affected. This phenomenon is
341:
James S. Nairne of Purdue University is the primary opponent of Thomson and Tulving's encoding specificity principle. He argues that the encoding-retrieval match is correlational rather than causal and states that many cognitive psychologists consider the principle to be "sacrosanct". Nairne
302:
This finding is a variation of the context-dependency effect of the encoding specificity principle and is much more apparent with low-imagery words than high-imagery words. Both high and low imagery words, however, are less likely to be recalled while intoxicated due to the inherent nature of
324:
easily recalled because of the mode of memory during the encoding process. This advertisement is a lasting example of emotional advertisements being easily recalled: it aired only once on September 7, 1964, yet is one of the most remembered and famous campaign advertisements to date.
354:
of the experiments failed to support the encoding specificity principle as strong extra-list cues facilitated the recall of tbr words in the presence of weak encoded cues and recall of the original weak encoded cues failed to be recognised in the context of new strong cues.
50:
The context may refer to the context in which the information was encoded, the physical location or surroundings, as well as the mental or physical state of the individual at the time of encoding. This principle plays a significant role in both the concept of
62:
Examples of the use of the encoding specificity principle include; studying in the same room as an exam is taken and the recall of information when intoxicated being easier when intoxicated again. 
741:
Grant, Harry; Lane C. Bredahl; John Clay; Jennifer Ferrie; Jane Groves; Timothy McDorman; Veronica Dark (1998). "Context-dependent memory for meaningful material: Information for students".
90:
of encoding. During a recall task, people benefit equally from a weakly related cue word as from a strongly related cue word, provided the weakly related word was present at encoding.
94:
researchers to discover their own individual findings. The following table shows the importance of priming through word pairs to achieve enhanced recall of words encoded together.
690: 694: 1072: 1006:
Weingartner, Herbert; Wolansa Adefras; James E. Eich; Dennis L. Murphy (1976). "Encoding-imagery specificity in alcohol state-dependent learning".
790: 666: 764: 1044:
Friestad, Marian; Esther Thorson (1993). "Remembering ads: the effects of encoding strategies, retrieval cues and emotional response".
315:
affects the rate of recall for the advertised product. When the nature of the advertisement was emotional, an encoding focus on
1319: 408:
Tulving, Endel; Thomson, Donald M. (1971). "Retrieval processes in recognition memory: Effects of associative context".
232:
Multiple studies have shown a dependence on context of one's environment as an aid to recall specific items and events.
857:
Granholm, Eric; Nelson Butters (1988). "Associative encoding and retrieval in Alzheimer's and Huntington's Disease".
1218:"Strong cues are not necessarily weak: Thomson and Tulving (1970) and the encoding specificity principle revisited" 630:
Godden, D.R.; A.D. Baddely (1975). "Context-Dependent Memory in Two Natural Environments: On Land and Underwater".
86:
related word is not present at the time of encoding, it will not be efficient at cuing recall for the target word.
23:
is the general principle that matching the encoding contexts of information at recall assists in the retrieval of
246: 52: 1076: 266: 1172: 750: 562:
Tulving, Endel; Donald Thomson (1973). "Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory".
370:
Tulving, Endel; Donald Thomson (1973). "Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory".
296: 284: 56: 320: 535:
Reder, Lynne; John Anderson; Robert Bjork (1974). "A semantic interpretation of encoding specificity".
1177: 755: 1198: 939: 882: 836: 811:
Marian, Viorica; Ulric Neisser (2000). "Language Dependent recall of autobiographical memories".
684: 579: 514: 387: 71: 1296: 1288: 1247: 1239: 1190: 1138: 1103: 1023: 988: 980: 931: 874: 828: 786: 672: 662: 506: 489:
Hannon, Brenda; Fergus Craik (2001). "Encoding specificity revisited: The role of semantics".
468: 460: 443:
Tulving, Endel; Osler, Shirley (1968). "Effectiveness of retrieval cues in memory for words".
425: 28: 1278: 1229: 1182: 1130: 1053: 1015: 970: 921: 913: 866: 820: 760: 720: 639: 609: 571: 544: 498: 452: 417: 379: 1121:
Mathews, C. O. (1938). "Comparison of methods of study for immediate and delayed recall".
316: 24: 711:
Godden, Duncan; Alan Baddely (1980). "When Does Context Influence Recognition Memory?".
900:
Adam, S.; M. Van der Linden; A. Ivanoiu; A.-C. Juillerat; S. Bechet; E. Salmon (2007).
724: 643: 312: 1057: 1313: 870: 43: 943: 902:"Optimization of encoding specificity for the diagnosis of early AD: The RI-48 task" 886: 583: 518: 391: 1202: 840: 36: 824: 350: 1186: 917: 1292: 1243: 1142: 1107: 1019: 984: 676: 659:
Cognitive psychology : connecting mind, research and everyday experience
464: 429: 27:. It provides a framework for understanding how the conditions present while 82: 1300: 1251: 1194: 935: 832: 765:
10.1002/(sici)1099-0720(1998120)12:6<617::aid-acp542>3.0.co;2-5
510: 1027: 992: 901: 878: 472: 1283: 1266: 1234: 1217: 975: 958: 926: 1163:
Nairne, James S. (2002). "The myth of the encoding-retrieval match".
1134: 613: 575: 548: 502: 456: 421: 383: 32: 1267:"Tests of the generality of the principle of encoding specificity" 262:
Language and the voluntary retrieval of autobiographical memories
600:
Bahrick, Harry (1970). "Two-phase model for prompted recall".
1008:
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory
852: 850: 806: 804: 802: 530: 528: 959:"The cue-dependent nature of state-dependent retrieval" 783:
Cognitive Psychology: Applying the Science of the mind
295:
Information encoded and stored while intoxicated, see
484: 482: 906:
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology
785:. Boston, MA: Pearson Publishing. pp. 246–248. 625: 623: 98:Paired-associate list and four types of prompters 736: 734: 595: 593: 706: 704: 1039: 1037: 8: 689:: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list ( 661:(4th ed.). New york: Cengage learning. 1075:. The Living Room Candidate. Archived from 776: 774: 491:Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 693:) CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list ( 1282: 1233: 1176: 1158: 1156: 1154: 1152: 974: 925: 754: 362: 682: 224:Modeled after Table 1 Bahrick (1970) 7: 403: 401: 657:Goldstein, E. Bruce, 1941- (2015). 1216:Higham, Philip A. (January 2002). 1100:Studying. Technical Report No. 155 813:Journal of Experimental Psychology 781:Robinson-Riegler, Bridget (2008). 725:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1980.tb02735.x 644:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1975.tb01468.x 537:Journal of Experimental Psychology 445:Journal of Experimental Psychology 410:Journal of Experimental Psychology 14: 1123:Journal of Educational Psychology 713:The British Journal of Psychology 632:The British Journal of Psychology 42:It was introduced by Thomson and 245:explained by what is termed the 957:Eich, James Eric (March 1980). 1265:Postman, Leo (November 1975). 1046:Journal of Consumer Psychology 21:encoding specificity principle 1: 1058:10.1016/s1057-7408(08)80072-1 16:Memory process-related theory 1071:Museum of the Moving Image. 871:10.1016/0278-2626(88)90007-3 743:Applied Cognitive Psychology 825:10.1037/0096-3445.129.3.361 96: 1336: 67:Development of the Concept 1187:10.1080/09658210244000216 918:10.1080/13803390600775339 267:Autobiographical memories 1020:10.1037/0278-7393.2.1.83 311:The emotional nature of 53:context-dependent memory 1271:Memory & Cognition 1222:Memory & Cognition 963:Memory & Cognition 297:state-dependent memory 57:state-dependent memory 31:information relate to 321:Political advertising 247:outshining hypothesis 39:of that information. 1320:Cognitive psychology 1098:Anderson, Thomas H. 602:Psychological Review 564:Psychological Review 372:Psychological Review 279:Diagnosis of disease 253:Auditory environment 236:Physical environment 195:communication (.33) 859:Brain and Cognition 285:Alzheimer's disease 99: 55:and the concept of 1284:10.3758/bf03198232 1235:10.3758/bf03195266 976:10.3758/bf03213419 97: 792:978-0-205-03364-5 668:978-1-285-76388-0 274:Specific Examples 228:Encoding Contexts 222: 221: 175:upholstery (.36) 78:Role of Semantics 25:episodic memories 1327: 1305: 1304: 1286: 1262: 1256: 1255: 1237: 1213: 1207: 1206: 1180: 1171:(5/6): 389–395. 1160: 1147: 1146: 1135:10.1037/h0055182 1118: 1112: 1111: 1095: 1089: 1088: 1086: 1084: 1079:on 26 April 2014 1068: 1062: 1061: 1041: 1032: 1031: 1003: 997: 996: 978: 954: 948: 947: 929: 897: 891: 890: 854: 845: 844: 808: 797: 796: 778: 769: 768: 758: 738: 729: 728: 708: 699: 698: 688: 680: 654: 648: 647: 627: 618: 617: 614:10.1037/h0029099 597: 588: 587: 576:10.1037/h0020071 559: 553: 552: 549:10.1037/h0036115 532: 523: 522: 503:10.1037/h0087369 486: 477: 476: 457:10.1037/h0026069 440: 434: 433: 422:10.1037/h0030186 405: 396: 395: 384:10.1037/h0020071 367: 192:extension (.17) 178:furniture (.48) 100: 1335: 1334: 1330: 1329: 1328: 1326: 1325: 1324: 1310: 1309: 1308: 1264: 1263: 1259: 1215: 1214: 1210: 1178:10.1.1.377.6640 1162: 1161: 1150: 1120: 1119: 1115: 1097: 1096: 1092: 1082: 1080: 1070: 1069: 1065: 1043: 1042: 1035: 1005: 1004: 1000: 956: 955: 951: 899: 898: 894: 856: 855: 848: 810: 809: 800: 793: 780: 779: 772: 756:10.1.1.497.6500 740: 739: 732: 710: 709: 702: 681: 669: 656: 655: 651: 629: 628: 621: 599: 598: 591: 561: 560: 556: 534: 533: 526: 488: 487: 480: 442: 441: 437: 407: 406: 399: 369: 368: 364: 360: 339: 330: 317:episodic memory 309: 293: 281: 276: 255: 238: 230: 215:feminine (.26) 80: 69: 17: 12: 11: 5: 1333: 1331: 1323: 1322: 1312: 1311: 1307: 1306: 1277:(6): 663–672. 1257: 1208: 1148: 1129:(2): 101–106. 1113: 1090: 1063: 1033: 998: 969:(2): 157–173. 949: 912:(5): 477–487. 892: 865:(3): 335–347. 846: 819:(3): 361–368. 798: 791: 770: 749:(6): 617–623. 730: 700: 667: 649: 638:(3): 325–331. 619: 608:(3): 215–222. 589: 570:(5): 352–373. 554: 543:(4): 648–656. 524: 497:(3): 231–243. 478: 451:(4): 593–601. 435: 416:(1): 116–124. 397: 378:(5): 352–373. 361: 359: 356: 338: 335: 329: 326: 313:advertisements 308: 305: 292: 289: 283:Patients with 280: 277: 275: 272: 254: 251: 237: 234: 229: 226: 220: 219: 216: 213: 210: 207: 204: 200: 199: 196: 193: 190: 187: 184: 180: 179: 176: 173: 172:cushion (.09) 170: 167: 164: 160: 159: 158:chapter (.59) 156: 153: 150: 147: 144: 140: 139: 136: 133: 130: 127: 124: 120: 119: 116: 113: 110: 107: 104: 79: 76: 68: 65: 15: 13: 10: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 1332: 1321: 1318: 1317: 1315: 1302: 1298: 1294: 1290: 1285: 1280: 1276: 1272: 1268: 1261: 1258: 1253: 1249: 1245: 1241: 1236: 1231: 1227: 1223: 1219: 1212: 1209: 1204: 1200: 1196: 1192: 1188: 1184: 1179: 1174: 1170: 1166: 1159: 1157: 1155: 1153: 1149: 1144: 1140: 1136: 1132: 1128: 1124: 1117: 1114: 1109: 1105: 1101: 1094: 1091: 1078: 1074: 1067: 1064: 1059: 1055: 1051: 1047: 1040: 1038: 1034: 1029: 1025: 1021: 1017: 1013: 1009: 1002: 999: 994: 990: 986: 982: 977: 972: 968: 964: 960: 953: 950: 945: 941: 937: 933: 928: 923: 919: 915: 911: 907: 903: 896: 893: 888: 884: 880: 876: 872: 868: 864: 860: 853: 851: 847: 842: 838: 834: 830: 826: 822: 818: 814: 807: 805: 803: 799: 794: 788: 784: 777: 775: 771: 766: 762: 757: 752: 748: 744: 737: 735: 731: 726: 722: 718: 714: 707: 705: 701: 696: 692: 686: 678: 674: 670: 664: 660: 653: 650: 645: 641: 637: 633: 626: 624: 620: 615: 611: 607: 603: 596: 594: 590: 585: 581: 577: 573: 569: 565: 558: 555: 550: 546: 542: 538: 531: 529: 525: 520: 516: 512: 508: 504: 500: 496: 492: 485: 483: 479: 474: 470: 466: 462: 458: 454: 450: 446: 439: 436: 431: 427: 423: 419: 415: 411: 404: 402: 398: 393: 389: 385: 381: 377: 373: 366: 363: 357: 355: 352: 347: 343: 336: 334: 327: 325: 322: 318: 314: 306: 304: 300: 298: 290: 288: 286: 278: 273: 271: 268: 264: 263: 259: 252: 250: 248: 242: 235: 233: 227: 225: 217: 214: 211: 208: 205: 202: 201: 197: 194: 191: 188: 185: 182: 181: 177: 174: 171: 168: 165: 162: 161: 157: 154: 151: 148: 145: 142: 141: 137: 134: 131: 129:velvet (.03) 128: 125: 122: 121: 117: 114: 111: 108: 105: 102: 101: 95: 91: 87: 84: 77: 75: 73: 66: 64: 60: 58: 54: 48: 45: 40: 38: 34: 30: 26: 22: 1274: 1270: 1260: 1228:(1): 67–80. 1225: 1221: 1211: 1168: 1164: 1126: 1122: 1116: 1099: 1093: 1081:. Retrieved 1077:the original 1066: 1049: 1045: 1014:(1): 83–87. 1011: 1007: 1001: 966: 962: 952: 909: 905: 895: 862: 858: 816: 812: 782: 746: 742: 716: 712: 658: 652: 635: 631: 605: 601: 567: 563: 557: 540: 536: 494: 490: 448: 444: 438: 413: 409: 375: 371: 365: 348: 344: 340: 331: 310: 301: 294: 282: 265: 261: 260: 256: 243: 239: 231: 223: 209:child (.03) 152:comic (.15) 149:print (.02) 138:azure (.58) 135:green (.28) 118:4 (.38-.59) 115:3 (.23-.36) 112:2 (.09-.21) 109:1 (.01-.08) 92: 88: 81: 70: 61: 49: 41: 20: 18: 1083:18 November 1052:(1): 1–23. 351:Leo Postman 307:Advertising 218:coed (.54) 212:cute (.18) 198:dial (.59) 189:pole (.04) 155:read (.35) 927:2268/28214 719:: 99–104. 358:References 186:telephone 169:leg (.02) 132:grey (.1) 72:Ebbinghaus 1293:0090-502X 1244:0090-502X 1173:CiteSeerX 1143:0022-0663 1108:967611520 985:0090-502X 751:CiteSeerX 685:cite book 677:885178247 465:0022-1015 430:0022-1015 337:Criticism 106:Response 103:Stimulus 83:Semantics 1314:Category 1301:24203908 1252:11958356 1195:12396651 944:31325865 936:17564913 887:20415261 833:11006905 584:14879511 519:17570987 511:11605558 392:14879511 349:In 1975 328:Studying 29:encoding 1203:8085159 1073:"Daisy" 1028:1249533 993:7382817 879:2969744 841:4107490 473:5672271 291:Alcohol 44:Tulving 1299:  1291:  1250:  1242:  1201:  1193:  1175:  1165:Memory 1141:  1106:  1026:  991:  983:  942:  934:  885:  877:  839:  831:  789:  753:  675:  665:  582:  517:  509:  471:  463:  428:  390:  166:chair 37:recall 33:memory 1199:S2CID 940:S2CID 883:S2CID 837:S2CID 580:S2CID 515:S2CID 388:S2CID 206:girl 203:TILE 183:WENT 146:book 143:SHOE 126:blue 123:TIME 1297:PMID 1289:ISSN 1248:PMID 1240:ISSN 1191:PMID 1139:ISSN 1104:OCLC 1085:2011 1024:PMID 989:PMID 981:ISSN 932:PMID 875:PMID 829:PMID 787:ISBN 695:link 691:link 673:OCLC 663:ISBN 507:PMID 469:PMID 461:ISSN 426:ISSN 163:TOP 35:and 19:The 1279:doi 1230:doi 1183:doi 1131:doi 1054:doi 1016:doi 971:doi 922:hdl 914:doi 867:doi 821:doi 817:129 761:doi 721:doi 640:doi 610:doi 572:doi 545:doi 541:102 499:doi 453:doi 418:doi 380:doi 1316:: 1295:. 1287:. 1273:. 1269:. 1246:. 1238:. 1226:30 1224:. 1220:. 1197:. 1189:. 1181:. 1169:10 1167:. 1151:^ 1137:. 1127:29 1125:. 1102:. 1048:. 1036:^ 1022:. 1010:. 987:. 979:. 965:. 961:. 938:. 930:. 920:. 910:29 908:. 904:. 881:. 873:. 861:. 849:^ 835:. 827:. 815:. 801:^ 773:^ 759:. 747:12 745:. 733:^ 717:71 715:. 703:^ 687:}} 683:{{ 671:. 636:66 634:. 622:^ 606:77 604:. 592:^ 578:. 568:80 566:. 539:. 527:^ 513:. 505:. 495:55 493:. 481:^ 467:. 459:. 449:77 447:. 424:. 414:87 412:. 400:^ 386:. 376:80 374:. 59:. 1303:. 1281:: 1275:3 1254:. 1232:: 1205:. 1185:: 1145:. 1133:: 1110:. 1087:. 1060:. 1056:: 1050:2 1030:. 1018:: 1012:2 995:. 973:: 967:8 946:. 924:: 916:: 889:. 869:: 863:7 843:. 823:: 795:. 767:. 763:: 727:. 723:: 697:) 679:. 646:. 642:: 616:. 612:: 586:. 574:: 551:. 547:: 521:. 501:: 475:. 455:: 432:. 420:: 394:. 382::

Index

episodic memories
encoding
memory
recall
Tulving
context-dependent memory
state-dependent memory
Ebbinghaus
Semantics
outshining hypothesis
Autobiographical memories
Alzheimer's disease
state-dependent memory
advertisements
episodic memory
Political advertising
Leo Postman
doi
10.1037/h0020071
S2CID
14879511


doi
10.1037/h0030186
ISSN
0022-1015
doi
10.1037/h0026069
ISSN

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.