Knowledge (XXG)

Paraphasia

Source πŸ“

337:
patient was instructed to look at twenty different phrases, each consisting of one to three syllables, then read the phrase. If the patient failed to read the phrase, the process was repeated. If the patient failed to read the phrase again, the process was abandoned. To progress from a set of one syllable phrases to two syllable phrases and two syllable phrases to three syllable phrases, an 80% success rate was necessary. This treatment was partially successful. Although fewer phonemic paraphasias were produced due to this treatment, speaking efficiency was not improved by this study. This is partially because the focus of the treatment was on sound production rather than semantic content. Improvements lasted for six weeks before the patient regressed.
333:. A traditional approach requires treatment beginning at the level of breakdown – in the case of paraphasia, at the level of the phoneme. There are commercially available workbooks that provide various activities such as letter, word-picture, or word-word matching, and sentence completion, among other things. The difficulty of these activities varies with the level of treatment. However, these treatments have not been proven to be clinically productive. 164:
which one word is replaced with another phonologically related to the intended word; phonemic ones, in which one word is replaced with a nonword phonologically related to the intended word; and approximations, an attempt to find the word without producing either a word or nonword. These types of errors are associated with Wernicke's aphasia, among others. Phonemic paraphasias are often caused by lesions to the
253:
preserve length, although the gender of the target word was preserved in more than half of the errors in one case study. It is hypothesized that verbal paraphasias are not the result of a random process but from a precise deficit in a single area. Verbal paraphasias are the only type of paraphasias that can also be linked to nonfluent aphasias, and they are mainly caused by lesions to the
176:. This type of paraphasia also occurs in other languages as well. For example, case studies have been performed with German speakers, which demonstrated that 30.8% of paraphasias occurred at the beginning of the word in patients with Wernicke's aphasia and 22.6% for patients with Broca's aphasia. In English speakers this tendency to create errors at the beginning of the word remained. 217:, and the previously mentioned pause can be used to indicate the relative severity of the neologism; less severe neologistic paraphasias can be recognized as a distortion of a real word, and more severe ones cannot. The hypothesized source for these neologisms is β€œa device which quasirandomly combines English 240:
There are also various types of neologistic paraphasias. They can be phonologically related to a prior word, a following word, the intended word, or another neologism. The neologistic paraphasia shares phonemes or the position of phonemes with the related word. This most often occurs when the word
100:
location is the most important determining factor for all aphasic disorders, including paraphasia – the location of the lesion can be used to hypothesize the type of aphasic symptoms the patient will display. This lesion can be caused by a variety of different methods: malfunctioning blood vessels
996:
T. Picht, S. M. Krieg, N. Sollmann, J. RΓΆsler, B. Niraula, T. Neuvonen, P. Savolainen, P. Lioumis, Pantelis, J. P. MΓ€kelΓ€, V. Deletis, B. Meyer, P. Vajkoczy, and F. Ringel, A comparison of language mapping by preoperative navigated transcranial magnetic stimulation and direct cortical stimulation
163:
Phonemic paraphasia, also referred to as phonological paraphasia or literal paraphasia, refers to the substitution of a word with a nonword that preserves at least half of the segments and/or number of syllables of the intended word. This can lead to a variety of errors, including formal ones, in
149:
Wernicke's aphasia is characterized by fluent language with made up or unnecessary words with little or no meaning to speech. Those who suffer from this type of aphasia have difficulty understanding others' speech and are unaware of their own mistakes. When corrected they will repeat their verbal
336:
A 1988 study by Mary Boyle proposed a method focused on oral reading to treat phonemic paraphasias, which was partially successful, resulting in fewer phonemic paraphasias but a slower rate of speech. Treatments lasted for 50 minutes and occurred once a week. During these treatment sessions, the
252:
Verbal paraphasias are confusions of words or the replacement of one word by another real word; another definition is that of a contextually inappropriate English word or an English word of a syntactically incorrect class – the wrong part of speech, for example. Verbal paraphasias do not often
192:
Addition errors, in which a segment is added that bears no relation to the intended word, are much rarer than anticipatory, perseverative, or paradigmatic errors, though they do occur. Most often what can be mistaken for an addition error is an anticipatory error from the words surrounding the
145:
is characterized by non-fluent or telegraphic-type speech - where articles, conjunctions, prepositions, auxiliary verbs, pronouns and morphological inflections (plurals, past tense) are omitted. The word substitutions are infrequent and distortion of consonants and simplification of consonant
241:
and neologistic paraphasia are in the same clause. Neologistic paraphasias have a less stringent relationship with the target word than phonological paraphasias – where a phonological paraphasia has more than half of the target word’s phonemes, a neologistic paraphasia has less than half.
316:
Many language impairments, including paraphasic errors, are reduced in number through spontaneous recovery of neurological function; this occurs most often with stroke patients within the first three months of recovery. Lesions associated with
303:
Perseverative paraphasia is a type of paraphasia in which the previous response persists and interferes with retrieval of new responses. (See the experimental case study D.L.A published by Dennis in 1976.) It is associated with lesions in the
150:
paraphasias and have trouble finding the correct word. Wernicke's area is found in the dominant hemisphere of the posterior gyrus of the first temporal convolution of the brain, whereas Broca's area is found anterior to the Wernicke's area.
96:, among others. All of these lead to a difference in processing efficiency, which is often caused by damage to a cortical region in the brain (in receptive aphasia, for example, the lesion is in or near Wernicke's area); 146:
clusters is frequent. Content words such as nouns, verbs and adjectives may be preserved. Subjects of this aphasia are aware of their errors in speech. Damage to the Broca's area does not affect comprehension of speech.
268:
These errors can be semantic, in which the meaning of the word is related to that of the intended word (car for van, for example). Semantic paraphasias can be further subdivided into six different types.
325:
have a longer period for spontaneous recovery, from four to eight weeks. Whether spontaneous recovery occurs or not, treatment must begin immediately after the stroke, with support from a
281:
Subordinate semantic paraphasias are the opposite of superordinate semantic paraphasias and replace the target word with one that is more specific – rose, for flower, for example.
141:
Two areas of the brain, Broca's area and Wernicke's area, are responsible for various disruptions in speech when damaged. Each is defined by their distinct characteristics.
349:(TMS). With navigated TMS (nTMS), nodes of the language network can be located presurgically so that critical areas can be saved when performing tumor or epilepsy surgery. 278:
Superordinate semantic paraphasias replace a specific target word with a more generalized group to which the target word involves, such as replacing pear with fruit.
987:
Boyle M. 1988. Reducing Phonemic Paraphasias in the Connected Speech of a Conduction Aphasic Subject. In Clinical Aphasiology Conference, pp. 379-93. Cape Cod, MA
284:
Part-whole semantic paraphasias replace the "whole" with the "part" as in finger for hand; or, conversely, the part with the whole, in the case of leg for foot.
183:
Anticipatory errors occur when a syllable from later in the word replaces a syllable from earlier in the word – "papple" for apple or "lelephone" for telephone.
37:, and characterized by the production of unintended syllables, words, or phrases during the effort to speak. Paraphasic errors are most common in patients with 345:
Transient paraphasias (as well as other language defects such as speech arrest) can be generated by artificially activating the brain's language network with
275:
Associate semantic paraphasias replace the target word with one that is related to the target but is not of the same category, like replacing foot with shoe.
186:
Perseverative errors occur when a syllable from earlier in the word replaces a syllable from later in the word – for example, "gingerjed" for gingerbread.
621:
Gallaher, Alan J. (Jun 24, 1981). "Syntactic versus semantic performances of agrammatic Broca's aphasics on tests of constituent-element-ordering".
213:
Neologistic paraphasias, a substitution with a non-English or gibberish word, follow pauses indicating word-finding difficulty. They can affect any
110: 53:, or both. Some paraphasias preserve the meter without segmentation, and some do the opposite. However, most paraphasias affect both partially. 287:
Finally, a visual semantic paraphasia replaces the target word with a word that shares visual features with the target, such as knife for nail.
76:
Paraphasia is associated with fluent aphasias, characterized by "fluent spontaneous speech, long grammatically shaped sentences and preserved
225:
resulting from aphasia, rather than any other of the sources with which they are associated. Neologistic paraphasia is often associated with
1024:"paraphasia - definition of paraphasia in the Medical dictionary - by the Free Online Medical Dictionary, Thesaurus and Encyclopedia" 346: 656:
Kellogg, Margaret Kimberly (2012). "Conceptual mechanisms underlying noun and verb categorization: Evidence from paraphasia".
510:
Sinanovic O, Mrkonjic Z, Zukic S, Vidovic M, Imamovic K. 2011. Post-stroke language disorders. Acta clinica Croatica 50:79-94
1030: 907: 93: 272:
Coordinate semantic paraphasias replace the target word with one that is from the same category, such as tiger for lion.
368: 768:"Analysis of naming errors during cortical stimulation mapping: Implications for models of language representation" 525:
Kreisler A, Godefroy O, Delmaire C, Debachy B, Leclercq M, et al. (2000). "The anatomy of aphasia revisited".
436:
Biran M, Friedmann N (2005). "From phonological paraphasias to the structure of the phonological output lexicon".
321:
have a shorter spontaneous recovery time, within the first two weeks; on the other hand, lesions associated with
189:
Paradigmatic errors based on similarity in how the sounds are formed can also occur – "marmer" for barber, i.e.
863:
Butterworth B (1979). "Hesitation and the production of verbal paraphasias and neologisms in jargon aphasia".
730:"The scan-copier mechanism and the positional level of language production: Evidence from phonemic paraphasia" 1065:
ScienceDaily - 'Weapons Of Mass Production', I Mean, 'Mass Destruction!' How The Brain Prevents Verbal Errors
64:, in a sentence reading, "In some cases there is a perfect chorea or delirium of words, which may be called 445: 126: 1091: 675:
Huber, Mary (1944). "A Phonetic Approach to the Problem of Perception in a Case of Wernicke's Aphasia".
77: 46: 42: 199:
Epenthetic errors are the insertion of a segment into the target, as in the case of "plants" for pants.
945:
Lewis FC, Soares L (2000). "Relationship between Semantic Paraphasias and Related Nonverbal Factors".
1011: 450: 122: 118: 488: 1070: 1064: 970: 888: 550: 463: 330: 142: 89: 193:
intended one. However, an example of a possible addition error could be "selezant" for elephant.
41:, and come in three forms: phonemic or literal, neologistic, and verbal. Paraphasias can affect 1086: 1053: 962: 880: 838: 797: 710: 638: 603: 542: 384: 363: 226: 81: 1023: 1045: 954: 922: 872: 828: 787: 779: 741: 702: 630: 593: 585: 534: 455: 326: 173: 165: 358: 305: 258: 196:
Substitution errors involve a clear phonological substitution, such as "ragon" for wagon.
105:) in the brain are the cause of 80% of aphasias in adults, as compared to head injuries, 482: 792: 767: 598: 573: 230: 214: 85: 57: 38: 1080: 1049: 876: 569: 254: 169: 130: 892: 554: 467: 202:
Finally, metathetical errors are the full exchange of segments like "deks" for desk.
389: 221:
in a phonotactically regular way.” A neologistic paraphasia can be thought of as a
17: 974: 927: 783: 766:
Corina DP, Loudermilk BC, Detwiler L, Martin RF, Brinkley JF, Ojemann G (2010).
706: 378: 373: 746: 729: 958: 459: 394: 833: 816: 222: 114: 966: 801: 714: 607: 589: 546: 292:
Random errors, in which the word has no relation to the target, also occur.
56:
The term was apparently introduced in 1877 by the German-English physician
842: 642: 634: 574:"Word-finding difficulty: a clinical analysis of the progressive aphasias" 538: 1057: 884: 218: 106: 50: 34: 1036:
Buckingham Hugh W., Rekart Deborah M. (1979). "Semantic paraphasia".
1031:"I Can't Come Up with the Right Word..." by William Barr, Ph.D., ABPP 658:
Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society
322: 318: 102: 97: 693:
Berg T (2006). "A structural account of phonological paraphasias".
906:
Garzon M, Semenza M, Meneghello F, Bencini G, Semenza C (2011).
1071:
Science Daily - Area Of Brain Key To Choosing Words Identified
484:
Diseases of the nervous system: their prevalence and pathology
33:
is a type of language output error commonly associated with
341:
Experimental (magnetic-stimulation-induced) paraphasias
997:
during awake surgery, Neurosurgery 72, 808–819 (2013).
80:
abilities." Examples of these fluent aphasias include
908:"Target-Unrelated Verbal Paraphasias: A Case Study" 420:Introduction to Neurogenic Communication Disorders 381:, similar to paraphasia, but for written languages 677:Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 858: 856: 854: 852: 8: 506: 504: 502: 500: 498: 133:are often associated with fluent aphasias. 520: 518: 516: 926: 915:Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 832: 791: 761: 759: 757: 745: 597: 568:Rohrer JD, Knight WD, Warren JE, Fox NC, 449: 168:, extending to the posterior part of the 27:Speech difficulty associated with aphasia 940: 938: 688: 686: 431: 429: 407: 623:Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 137:Damage to the brain's language centers 817:"Inflection of neologisms in aphasia" 7: 815:Caplan D, Kellar L, Locke S (1972). 413: 411: 397:, an unintelligible jumble of words 1038:Journal of Communication Disorders 1016:The Neuroscience on the Web Series 25: 347:Transcranial magnetic stimulation 237:Types of Neologistic paraphasias 438:Language and Cognitive Processes 136: 131:posterior superior temporal lobe 82:receptive or Wernicke's aphasia 257:of the brain, the head of the 180:Types of phonemic paraphasias 62:Diseases of the Nervous System 1: 94:transcortical sensory aphasia 1050:10.1016/0021-9924(79)90041-8 928:10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.09.210 877:10.1016/0093-934x(79)90046-4 947:Perceptual and Motor Skills 784:10.1016/j.bandl.2010.04.001 707:10.1016/j.bandl.2006.01.005 369:Lists of language disorders 101:(caused, for example, by a 1108: 1012:"Aphasia: Characteristics" 747:10.1207/s15516709cog1002_4 959:10.2466/pms.2000.91.2.366 460:10.1080/01690960400005813 255:posterior temporal region 487:. Smith, Elder. p.  418:Manasco, Hunter (2014). 299:Perseverative paraphasia 129:. Lesions involving the 481:Julius Althaus (1877). 39:fluent forms of aphasia 834:10.1093/brain/95.1.169 728:Buckingham HW (1986). 209:Neologistic paraphasia 127:demyelinating diseases 635:10.1044/jshr.2402.217 539:10.1212/wnl.54.5.1117 111:degenerative diseases 47:segmental information 590:10.1093/brain/awm251 572:, Warren JD (2008). 306:left caudate nucleus 43:metrical information 323:hemorrhagic strokes 159:Phonemic paraphasia 123:infectious diseases 119:metabolic disorders 51:number of syllables 18:Semantic paraphasia 865:Brain and Language 772:Brain and Language 695:Brain and Language 331:speech pathologist 90:conduction aphasia 734:Cognitive Science 385:Speech disfluency 364:Language disorder 248:Verbal paraphasia 227:receptive aphasia 16:(Redirected from 1099: 1061: 1027: 1019: 998: 994: 988: 985: 979: 978: 942: 933: 932: 930: 912: 903: 897: 896: 860: 847: 846: 836: 812: 806: 805: 795: 763: 752: 751: 749: 725: 719: 718: 690: 681: 680: 672: 666: 665: 653: 647: 646: 618: 612: 611: 601: 565: 559: 558: 522: 511: 508: 493: 492: 478: 472: 471: 453: 433: 424: 423: 415: 327:speech therapist 319:ischemic strokes 174:internal capsule 166:external capsule 21: 1107: 1106: 1102: 1101: 1100: 1098: 1097: 1096: 1077: 1076: 1035: 1022: 1010: 1007: 1002: 1001: 995: 991: 986: 982: 944: 943: 936: 910: 905: 904: 900: 862: 861: 850: 814: 813: 809: 765: 764: 755: 727: 726: 722: 692: 691: 684: 674: 673: 669: 655: 654: 650: 620: 619: 615: 567: 566: 562: 524: 523: 514: 509: 496: 480: 479: 475: 435: 434: 427: 417: 416: 409: 404: 359:Ganser syndrome 355: 343: 314: 301: 259:caudate nucleus 250: 211: 161: 156: 143:Broca's aphasia 139: 74: 60:in his book on 28: 23: 22: 15: 12: 11: 5: 1105: 1103: 1095: 1094: 1089: 1079: 1078: 1075: 1074: 1073:, Dec 30, 2008 1068: 1062: 1044:(3): 197–209. 1033: 1028: 1020: 1006: 1003: 1000: 999: 989: 980: 934: 898: 848: 807: 753: 740:(2): 195–217. 720: 682: 667: 648: 629:(2): 217–218. 613: 584:(Pt 1): 8–38. 560: 533:(5): 1117–23. 512: 494: 473: 451:10.1.1.69.7795 444:(4): 589–616. 425: 406: 405: 403: 400: 399: 398: 392: 387: 382: 376: 371: 366: 361: 354: 351: 342: 339: 313: 310: 300: 297: 296: 295: 294: 293: 290: 289: 288: 285: 282: 279: 276: 273: 249: 246: 245: 244: 243: 242: 231:jargon aphasia 215:part of speech 210: 207: 206: 205: 204: 203: 200: 197: 194: 190: 187: 184: 160: 157: 155: 152: 138: 135: 86:anomic aphasia 73: 70: 58:Julius Althaus 26: 24: 14: 13: 10: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 1104: 1093: 1090: 1088: 1085: 1084: 1082: 1072: 1069: 1067:, Nov 5, 2008 1066: 1063: 1059: 1055: 1051: 1047: 1043: 1039: 1034: 1032: 1029: 1025: 1021: 1017: 1013: 1009: 1008: 1004: 993: 990: 984: 981: 976: 972: 968: 964: 960: 956: 953:(2): 366–72. 952: 948: 941: 939: 935: 929: 924: 920: 916: 909: 902: 899: 894: 890: 886: 882: 878: 874: 871:(2): 133–61. 870: 866: 859: 857: 855: 853: 849: 844: 840: 835: 830: 827:(1): 169–72. 826: 822: 818: 811: 808: 803: 799: 794: 789: 785: 781: 778:(2): 101–12. 777: 773: 769: 762: 760: 758: 754: 748: 743: 739: 735: 731: 724: 721: 716: 712: 708: 704: 701:(3): 331–56. 700: 696: 689: 687: 683: 678: 671: 668: 663: 659: 652: 649: 644: 640: 636: 632: 628: 624: 617: 614: 609: 605: 600: 595: 591: 587: 583: 579: 575: 571: 564: 561: 556: 552: 548: 544: 540: 536: 532: 528: 521: 519: 517: 513: 507: 505: 503: 501: 499: 495: 490: 486: 485: 477: 474: 469: 465: 461: 457: 452: 447: 443: 439: 432: 430: 426: 422:. p. 73. 421: 414: 412: 408: 401: 396: 393: 391: 388: 386: 383: 380: 377: 375: 372: 370: 367: 365: 362: 360: 357: 356: 352: 350: 348: 340: 338: 334: 332: 328: 324: 320: 311: 309: 307: 298: 291: 286: 283: 280: 277: 274: 271: 270: 267: 266: 264: 263: 262: 260: 256: 247: 239: 238: 236: 235: 234: 232: 228: 224: 220: 216: 208: 201: 198: 195: 191: 188: 185: 182: 181: 179: 178: 177: 175: 171: 170:temporal lobe 167: 158: 153: 151: 147: 144: 134: 132: 128: 124: 120: 116: 112: 108: 104: 99: 95: 91: 87: 83: 79: 71: 69: 67: 63: 59: 54: 52: 48: 44: 40: 36: 32: 19: 1092:Speech error 1041: 1037: 1015: 992: 983: 950: 946: 918: 914: 901: 868: 864: 824: 820: 810: 775: 771: 737: 733: 723: 698: 694: 676: 670: 661: 657: 651: 626: 622: 616: 581: 577: 563: 530: 526: 483: 476: 441: 437: 419: 390:Speech error 344: 335: 315: 302: 251: 212: 162: 148: 140: 75: 65: 61: 55: 30: 29: 379:Paragraphia 374:Malapropism 261:, or both. 1081:Categories 679:: 227–238. 402:References 395:Word salad 66:paraphasia 31:Paraphasia 921:: 142–3. 570:Rossor MN 527:Neurology 446:CiteSeerX 312:Treatment 265:Examples 223:neologism 115:poisoning 1087:Aphasias 967:11065295 893:39339730 802:20452661 715:16615177 608:17947337 555:21847976 547:10720284 468:14484194 353:See also 219:phonemes 154:Symptoms 107:dementia 1005:Sources 843:5023086 793:3247200 643:7265937 599:2373641 78:prosody 35:aphasia 1058:438359 1056:  975:755716 973:  965:  891:  885:487066 883:  841:  800:  790:  713:  641:  606:  596:  553:  545:  466:  448:  125:, and 103:stroke 98:lesion 92:, and 72:Causes 971:S2CID 911:(PDF) 889:S2CID 821:Brain 578:Brain 551:S2CID 464:S2CID 1054:PMID 963:PMID 881:PMID 839:PMID 798:PMID 711:PMID 664:(1). 639:PMID 604:PMID 543:PMID 229:and 109:and 1046:doi 955:doi 923:doi 873:doi 829:doi 788:PMC 780:doi 776:115 742:doi 703:doi 631:doi 594:PMC 586:doi 582:131 535:doi 489:163 456:doi 329:or 172:or 68:". 1083:: 1052:. 1042:12 1040:. 1014:. 969:. 961:. 951:91 949:. 937:^ 919:23 917:. 913:. 887:. 879:. 867:. 851:^ 837:. 825:95 823:. 819:. 796:. 786:. 774:. 770:. 756:^ 738:10 736:. 732:. 709:. 699:96 697:. 685:^ 662:20 660:. 637:. 627:24 625:. 602:. 592:. 580:. 576:. 549:. 541:. 531:54 529:. 515:^ 497:^ 462:. 454:. 442:20 440:. 428:^ 410:^ 308:. 233:. 121:, 117:, 113:, 88:, 84:, 49:, 45:, 1060:. 1048:: 1026:. 1018:. 977:. 957:: 931:. 925:: 895:. 875:: 869:8 845:. 831:: 804:. 782:: 750:. 744:: 717:. 705:: 645:. 633:: 610:. 588:: 557:. 537:: 491:. 470:. 458:: 20:)

Index

Semantic paraphasia
aphasia
fluent forms of aphasia
metrical information
segmental information
number of syllables
Julius Althaus
prosody
receptive or Wernicke's aphasia
anomic aphasia
conduction aphasia
transcortical sensory aphasia
lesion
stroke
dementia
degenerative diseases
poisoning
metabolic disorders
infectious diseases
demyelinating diseases
posterior superior temporal lobe
Broca's aphasia
external capsule
temporal lobe
internal capsule
part of speech
phonemes
neologism
receptive aphasia
jargon aphasia

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.

↑