Knowledge (XXG)

Visual variable

Source đź“ť

319: 215: 472: 198:
importance. Studies have shown that humans are better at judging relative differences in linear distance (e.g. one road being twice as thick as another) than relative differences in area (e.g., one circle having twice the area of another). Such estimations are the most accurate from squares. Area differences of circles are generally underestimated, but there is a large variation between people in ability to estimate two-dimensional size. Correctly estimating relative volume has proven even more difficult.
280: 131:, citing neither Bertin nor Robinson but saying that it was a "traditional categorization," suggesting its widespread nature by that point. Several terms were proposed for this set of categories, including Bertin's "retinal variables" (used to distinguish them from his two spatial location variables), as well as "Graphic Variables," "Symbol Dimensions," and "Primary Graphic Elements," before eventually settling on "Visual Variables," as used almost universally (in English) today. 186: 223:
conveyed. Some aspects of shape are inherent to the phenomenon and may not be easily manipulable, especially in line and region symbols, such as the shape of a road or a country. However, shape can still play a role in line and region symbols, such as a region filled with tree symbols or an arrowhead on a line. Also, the shape of a feature may be purposefully distorted by
366: 438:. This was a core part of Bertin's model, who distinguished these "imposition variables" from the other "retinal variables." This has largely been dropped from most subsequent lists by cartographers, since location in a map is predetermined by geography. However, it is crucial for representing information in 613:
are those with sufficiently strong variation so that the reader can isolate one value from all the others (e.g., "where are all the blue points?" amid points of various hues). All of the dissociative variables are also selective, plus Pattern Spacing and Hue (and Pattern Arrangement post-1967). These
596:
have variations that can be mentally subdued so they are easily grouped together, with none naturally standing out from the others so they do not contribute much to visual hierarchy. Bertin included Shape, Orientation, Color (Hue), and Grain (Pattern spacing) in this list; Of the post-1967 variables,
620:
show a clear linear order between different values. Bertin listed Size, Value, and Grain as ordered; later ordered variables would include Height, Saturation, Transparency, Crispness, and Resolution. This ordering makes them useful for representing Ordinal and Interval data. Hue and Orientation are
528:
a symbol or feature as a way of generalizing and obscuring it, usually for communicating some form of uncertainty about the feature. This was also first introduced in that context by MacEachren, but is not commonly used, and has rarely been mentioned since. By extension, this can also refer to the
462:
In a regular arrangement, the direction in which the sub-symbols are arrayed. Bertin considered this just the area version of the primary variable of orientation, but Morrison included it as a separate variable, likely because the orientation of the individual sub-symbols may be different than the
388:
in this context generally refers to an aggregate symbol composed of recurring sub-symbols. This can include areas (such as a forest filled with small tree point symbols) and line symbols (such as a railroad with recurring cross-hatches). These sub-symbols can themselves be created by any or all of
336:
of a color is its purity or intensity, created by the variety of light composing it; a single wavelength of light is of the highest saturation, while white, black, or gray has no saturation (being an even mixture of all visible wavelengths). Of the three psychological aspects of color, this is the
817:
from pale yellow to dark green, using both hue and value. Alternatively, different visual variables may be used to represent different properties; for example, symbols for cities may be differentiated by size to indicate population, and by shape to indicate provincial and national capitals. Some
222:
A shape is a simple design that is used to symbolize an attribute on a map. Shape is most commonly attached to point features in maps. Some shapes are simple in nature and thus are more abstract, while other shapes are more pictorial and are easy for the reader to comprehend what is trying to be
143:
or any other, and was primarily a practical summation of patterns he found in practice. The "truth" of the visual variables concept was largely established by its widespread and long-lasting acceptance. Thirty years later, MacEachren connected the scientific support for this and other aspects of
417:
The orderliness of the location of the sub-symbols in the pattern, generally either regularly spaced in rows and columns (often indicating a human construction, such as an orchard), or randomly spaced (often indicating a natural distribution). This variable first appears in Morrison's 1974 list
197:
The size of a symbol is how much space it occupies. This commonly refers to the area of point symbols, and the thickness of line symbols. Size differences are relatively easy to recognize, making it a useful variable to convey information, such as a quantitative amount of something, or relative
491:
These corollary terms refer to the degree to which a symbol blends with other symbols at the same location, giving the illusion of the symbol in front being translucent. A fairly recent addition, the control of opacity has become common in digital cartography. While it is rarely used to convey
167:
The earliest lists commonly suggested six variables: location size, shape, value, hue, orientation, and grain (pattern spacing). To this list, several additions have been suggested, with a few entering the canonical lists found in textbooks, while other suggestions have largely been dropped in
356:
Orientation refers to the direction labels and symbols are facing on a map (occasionally called "direction" or "angle"). Although it is not used as often as many of the other visual variables, it can be useful for communicating information about the real-world orientation of features. Common
604:
have variations that are harder to ignore, because some values stand out much more than others; therefore, they play a strong role in the visual hierarchy. Size and Value are the original members of this group, 3-D Height, Color Saturation, Transparency, Crispness, and Resolution are also
581:(e.g., size: Large-small ~ more-less). These modes make each variable better for representing certain kinds of information, and serving certain purposes, than others. Specifically, Bertin introduces four properties of these variables, which tie them directly to their role in the 138:
was the first systematic and theoretical treatment, and his overall approach to graphical symbolization is still in use today with only minor modifications. Despite the title of Bertin's work, it actually contained little reference to the scientific knowledge in the field of
369:
Texture (dot density) representing disease incidence (a ratio or count-level variable), which gives the appearance of density. Also, saturation (color vs. gray) is used to create a visual hierarchy, and value (gray vs. white) establishes a figure-ground contrast for
812:
Map symbols commonly employ multiple visual variables simultaneously. This can be used to reinforce the depiction of a single property; for example, a capital city having a symbol that is larger and a different shape than other cities, or a color progression on a
95:(EPHE) in Paris, where he created maps and graphics for faculty from various disciplines using a wide variety of data. Seeing recurring patterns, he created a system for symbolizing qualitative and quantitative information, apparently inspired by the sciences of 283:
Population density (a ratio-level variable) represented as color value, with an intuitive correspondence (i.e., dark looks like more people). Value also establishes figure-ground (color vs. white). Hue does not carry information here, but serves an aesthetic
271:, and others. Maps often use hue to differentiate categories of nominal variables, such as land cover types or geologic layers. Hue is also often used for its psychological connotations, such as red implying heat or danger and blue implying cold or water. 627:
have values that can be directly measured, and are thus best for representing quantitative properties, especially Ratio-level. Bertin included only size in this variable, although some would argue that value is quantitative, if less easily measured than
201:
Because geographical features have an actual size on the Earth, this cannot always be controlled, and sometimes works against the wishes of a cartographer; for example, it can be difficult to make a world map in which Russia does not stand out. In a
43:
Bertin identified a basic set of these variables and provided guidance for their usage; the concept and the set of variables has since been expanded, especially in cartography, where it has become a core principle of education and practice.
176:
Starting with Robinson and Bertin, a core set of visual variables has become largely canonical, appearing in cartography and information visualization textbooks, and built into most design software in some form.
818:
visual variable can be combined harmoniously to make a map clearer and more informative, while other combinations tend to add more confusion than usefulness. For example, early experiments with using
568:: Location, Loudness, Pitch, Register, Timbre, Duration, Rate of Change, Order (sequential), Attack/Decay. To date, sound has been rarely used to encode information in maps and information displays. 88:, which quickly became the dominant textbook on the subject, discussed size, shape, color, and pattern as the qualities of map symbols that establish contrast and represent geographic information. 614:
are generally better for representing precise information than non-selective variables (Orientation and Shape, although even they can be made selective if they are made distinct enough).
35:, is an aspect of a graphical object that can visually differentiate it from other objects, and can be controlled during the design process. The concept was first systematized by 463:
angle in which they are arranged. In recent years, it has rarely been included, likely due to the overall decrease in the use of fill patterns in the era of digital cartography.
348:
Bertin mentions saturation in his discussion of "color" (hue), but did not include it as a distinct variable. However, it has been included in almost all lists since the 1970s
426:
A number of additional variables have been suggested at times. Some are recent technology-driven proposals, while others are earlier entries that have fallen out of favor.
549:
Following on the widespread usefulness of Bertin's variables, cartographers have proposed analogous sets of controllable variables for media beyond static paper maps:
492:
specific information, it is effective for reducing contrast or to retain underlying information. Despite its widespread use, it is rarely mentioned in textbooks.
298:
refers to how light or dark an object appears. Value effectively connotes "more" and "less," an ordinal measure; this makes it a very useful form of symbology in
310:; elements that contrast most with the value of the background tend to stand out most (e.g., black on a white sheet of paper, white on a black computer screen). 556:: Duration, Order/sequence, Rate of change, Display time, Frequency of change, Synchronization (of multiple series). Many of these have entered mainstream use. 597:
Pattern Orientation and Arrangement are also associative, while Color Saturation is a possibility. These are well-suited for representing nominal variables.
577:
According to Bertin, each of the visual variables suggests its own mode of perception and interpretation, which MacEachren ties to the cognitive theory of
446:. Even in cartography, position becomes a variable when labeling and laying out the non-map elements on the page. It is also relevant when representing 322:
The synergy of Saturation (color vs. gray), value (dark vs. light), and position (centrality) to strongly establish figure-ground and visual hierarchy
153: 60:
to represent non-visual information since the 17th Century, and information visualization blossomed in the 19th Century, highlighted by the work of
562:: Vibration, Flutter, Pressure, Temperature, Resistance, Friction, Location, Height/Elevation, and analogues of most of the core visual variables. 168:
cartography. With the rise of multimedia as a cartographic tool, analogous sets of non-visual communication variables have also been presented.
92: 1193:. ACM International Conference Proceeding Series; Vol. 142, Proceedings of the Asia-Pacific symposium on Information visualisation - Volume 24 401:
was translated as "texture" in the 1983 English edition, and appeared frequently as such in subsequent lists, but others have suggested that
1121:
DiBiase, D., MacEachren, A. M., Krygier, J. B., & Reeves, C. (1992). Animation and the role of map design in scientific visualization.
802: 338: 801:
Each of these variables may be employed to convey information, to provide contrast between different features and layers, to establish
318: 1219: 240: 1038: 1005: 541:
On three-dimensional perspective maps, it is common to extrude shapes in the z direction, so that height represents a property.
997: 134:
Bertin has largely been given credit for the system of visual variables; even though he was not the first to mention the idea,
1190: 1092: 621:
ordered; not in a typical "more-less" metaphor, but in a cyclical order. Thus they can be used to represent cyclical data.
486: 84:
published an early version of his list of visual variables: shape, value, and "sparkling" (grain). Robinson, in his 1960
951:
Morrison, Joel, A Theoretical Framework for Cartographic Generalization With Emphasis on the Process of Symbolization,
530: 435: 224: 128: 632:
Bertin's classification is rarely mentioned as such, but the resultant applicability preferences form a core part of
218:
National Park standard point symbols, using shape to represent different types of facilities, a nominal variable.
120: 111:. Despite having a background in cartography, and deriving many of his ideas by evaluating maps, he intended for 442:
and other data visualizations; for example, position is the main method of visualizing quantitative values in a
231:, although this distortion is rarely used to convey information, only to reduce emphasis on shape and location. 214: 471: 1214: 1147:
Griffin, A. L. (2001). Feeling it out: the use of haptic visualization for exploratory geographic analysis.
529:
general level of detail in a symbol, which is used more often than pixelation, especially in the context of
80:
discussed the role of size, shape, and color in establishing contrast in maps. At the same time in France,
876: 375: 65: 916:
Palsky, Gilles (2019) Jacques Bertin, from classical training to systematic thinking of graphic signs,
279: 868: 1209: 721: 647: 641: 586: 68:. However, the direct study of this abstract use of graphical appearance began with the emergence of 1191:"An empirical evaluation of Chernoff faces, star glyphs, and spatial visualizations for binary data" 24: 500:
This is the degree to which a symbol is drawn with crisp or fuzzy edges. Briefly mentioned in the
781: 762: 702: 157: 104: 77: 32: 1022: 475:
Transparency and fuzziness is used effectively here to indicate overlapping sovereignty claims.
107:(it is sometimes hard to tell because his early works rarely cite any sources), culminating in 871:, in D. Richardson, N. Castree, M.F. Goodchild, A. Kobayashki, W. Liu, and R.A. Marston, eds. 447: 185: 454:
is an abstract visualization of a property, not the location of a real-world linear feature.
806: 637: 582: 480: 342: 337:
least effective at conveying specific information, but it is very effective at establishing
307: 61: 505: 849:, English Edition, Translation by William J. Berg, University of Wisconsin Press, 1983.) 206:
the size of features is purposefully distorted to represent a variable other than area.
814: 397:
The amount of white space between the sub-symbols in the pattern. Bertin's French term
303: 116: 81: 36: 28: 1203: 819: 345:, with bright colors generally standing out more than muted tones or shades of gray. 124: 1064: 1009: 636:, including the power of Size, Value, Saturation, and Resolution for establishing a 578: 451: 327: 299: 259:
is the visual perceptual property corresponding in humans to the categories called
161: 100: 1049: 741: 443: 228: 69: 796: 633: 525: 508:
in 1992 as a tool for representing locational uncertainty; he first called it
389:
the above visual variables, but a few variables apply to the overall pattern:
39:, a French cartographer and graphic designer, and published in his 1967 book, 1152: 1180:. A. M. MacEachren and D. R. F. Taylor (Eds.). Oxford: Pergamon, pp. 149–166 289: 203: 190: 149: 140: 96: 357:
examples include wind direction and the direction in which a spring flows.
1165:
Proceedings, 15th Conference of the International Cartographic Association
1108:
Slocum, Terry A., Robert B. McMaster, Fritz C. Kessler, Hugh H. Howard,
365: 434:
The absolute location of the symbol in the design, specified as (x,y)
76:(1952), often considered the genesis of American cartographic theory, 1163:
Vasconcellos, R. (1992) Knowing the Amazon through tactual graphics.
822:
on maps have been criticized as difficult to interpret correctly.
470: 439: 380:
Although terminology for this aspect still varies somewhat today,
364: 317: 278: 227:, especially when creating schematic representations such as many 213: 193:
representing population (a ratio or count-level property) by size.
184: 57: 72:
as an academic research discipline in the mid-20th Century. In
1176:
Krygier, J. B. (1994). Sound and geographic visualization. In
251: 123:. Soon the idea was gaining international acceptance; in 1974 53: 843:
Sémiologie Graphique. Les diagrammes, les réseaux, les cartes
585:
and to their ability to represent data in each of Steven's
516:, which has been the most common term in subsequent lists. 1136:
Some truth with maps: A primer on symbolization and design
504:
textbook in 1978, the concept was more fully developed by
985:
How Maps Work: Representation, Visualization, and Design
1189:
Michael D. Lee, Rachel E. Reilly, Marcus E. Butavicius
715:
Color value, Color saturation, Transparency, Crispness
1075:
Robinson, Arthur, Randall D. Sale, Joel L. Morrison,
845:. With Marc Barbut . Paris : Gauthier-Villars. 920:, 46:2, 189-193, DOI: 10.1080/15230406.2018.1523026 127:presented a very similar system in the context of 1112:, 3rd Edition, Pearson-Prentice Hall, 2009, p.84. 1087: 1085: 1065:https://www.e-education.psu.edu/geog486/node/1864 979: 977: 1123:Cartography and geographic information systems 947: 945: 943: 941: 939: 918:Cartography and Geographic Information Science 809:, or add to the aesthetic appeal of the map. 8: 963: 961: 873:The International Encyclopedia of Geography 863: 861: 859: 857: 855: 1110:Thematic Cartography and Geovisualization 164:, and 40 years of cartographic research. 154:Semiotic theory of Charles Sanders Peirce 998:"Graduated and Proportional Symbol Maps" 912: 910: 837: 835: 718:Size, Height, Color Hue, Pattern Spacing 645: 16:Graphic techniques used in visual design 1002:GEOG 486: Cartography and Visualization 831: 1138:. Association of American Geographers. 524:This is the technique of purposefully 953:International Yearbook of Cartography 640:, and the following ties to Steven's 52:Graphic techniques have been used in 7: 738:Size, Color saturation, Opacity, Hue 1178:Visualization in Modern Cartography 1054:Westfaelische Wilhelms Universitaet 904:University of Wisconsin Press, 1952 877:doi:10.1002/9781118786352.wbieg0761 969:Cartographic Design and Production 450:; for example, the location of an 314:Color: saturation/chroma/intensity 241:Color coding in data visualization 14: 1093:Visualizing Uncertain Information 1006:The Pennsylvania State University 732:Amount of quantitative difference 91:Bertin was a cartographer at the 692:Degree of qualitative difference 681:Pattern Arrangement, Orientation 306:. Value contributes strongly to 148:, bringing together research in 93:École pratique des hautes Ă©tudes 891:. New York: The Guilford Press. 115:to be applied to all forms of 1: 1167:, Bournemouth, UK, pp.206-210 758:Transparency, Pattern Spacing 487:Transparency and translucency 724:(rich, middle class, poor) 531:cartographic generalization 225:Cartographic generalization 129:cartographic generalization 1236: 1134:MacEachren, A. M. (1994). 794: 484: 478: 373: 325: 287: 249: 238: 1220:Information visualization 1149:Cartographic Perspectives 1097:Cartographic Perspectives 1079:, 4th Edition, Wiley 1978 755:Size, Height, Color value 409:is a better translation. 121:information visualization 889:Principles of map design 791:Use in map symbolization 761:Population growth rate, 1077:Elements in Cartography 971:, London: Longman, 1973 931:Elements of Cartography 752:Proportional difference 573:Visualizing Information 294:As an aspect of color, 144:cartographic design in 86:Elements of Cartography 1153:DOI: 10.14714/CP39.636 1099:, 13 (Fall 1992), p.10 1039:"Cartographic Symbols" 803:figure-ground contrast 775:Color hue, orientation 625:Quantitative variables 602:Dissociative variables 476: 371: 323: 285: 275:Color: value/lightness 219: 194: 1091:MacEachren, Alan M., 983:MacEachren, Alan M., 887:Tyner, J. A. (2010). 847:Semiology of Graphics 684:Owner, Facility type 650:and Visual Variables 642:levels of measurement 594:Associative variables 587:levels of measurement 554:Dynamic/animated maps 474: 376:Texture (visual arts) 368: 321: 282: 217: 188: 172:Core visual variables 66:Charles Joseph Minard 41:SĂ©miologie Graphique. 955:, V.14 (1974), p.115 933:, Wiley, 1960, p.137 722:Socioeconomic status 545:Non-visual variables 467:Transparency/opacity 422:Additional Variables 136:SĂ©miologie Graphique 113:SĂ©miologie Graphique 109:SĂ©miologie Graphique 661:Preferred Variables 651: 611:Selective variables 560:Haptic (touch) maps 496:Crispness/fuzziness 458:Pattern Orientation 25:cartographic design 1050:"Visual Variables" 929:Robinson, Arthur, 900:Robinson, Arthur, 772:Angular difference 763:population density 703:Geologic formation 698:Shape, Arrangement 664:Marginal Variables 646: 477: 372: 324: 286: 220: 195: 158:Gestalt psychology 105:Gestalt psychology 78:Arthur H. Robinson 58:statistical charts 33:data visualization 1125:, 19(4), 201–214. 967:Keates, John S., 902:The Look of Maps, 788: 787: 780:Day of the year, 675:Same or different 618:Ordered variables 1227: 1194: 1187: 1181: 1174: 1168: 1161: 1155: 1145: 1139: 1132: 1126: 1119: 1113: 1106: 1100: 1089: 1080: 1073: 1067: 1062: 1056: 1047: 1041: 1035: 1029: 1020: 1014: 1013: 1008:. Archived from 994: 988: 987:, Guilford, 1995 981: 972: 965: 956: 949: 934: 927: 921: 914: 905: 898: 892: 885: 879: 869:Visual Variables 867:Roth, Robert E. 865: 850: 839: 807:visual hierarchy 678:Color Hue, Shape 652: 638:visual hierarchy 583:visual hierarchy 481:Opacity (optics) 343:visual hierarchy 308:Visual hierarchy 152:(especially the 74:The Look of Maps 62:William Playfair 1235: 1234: 1230: 1229: 1228: 1226: 1225: 1224: 1200: 1199: 1198: 1197: 1188: 1184: 1175: 1171: 1162: 1158: 1151:, (39), 12–29. 1146: 1142: 1133: 1129: 1120: 1116: 1107: 1103: 1090: 1083: 1074: 1070: 1063: 1059: 1048: 1044: 1037:Symbol Basics, 1036: 1032: 1021: 1017: 996: 995: 991: 982: 975: 966: 959: 950: 937: 928: 924: 915: 908: 899: 895: 886: 882: 875:, Wiley, 2016. 866: 853: 841:Jacque Bertin, 840: 833: 828: 799: 793: 575: 547: 539: 522: 506:Alan MacEachren 498: 489: 483: 469: 460: 432: 424: 415: 395: 378: 363: 361:Pattern/Texture 354: 330: 316: 304:choropleth maps 292: 277: 254: 248: 243: 237: 212: 183: 174: 50: 21:visual variable 17: 12: 11: 5: 1233: 1231: 1223: 1222: 1217: 1215:Graphic design 1212: 1202: 1201: 1196: 1195: 1182: 1169: 1156: 1140: 1127: 1114: 1101: 1081: 1068: 1057: 1042: 1030: 1027:GIS Dictionary 1015: 1012:on 2017-07-13. 989: 973: 957: 935: 922: 906: 893: 880: 851: 830: 829: 827: 824: 820:Chernoff faces 815:choropleth map 795:Main article: 792: 789: 786: 785: 778: 776: 773: 770: 766: 765: 759: 756: 753: 750: 746: 745: 739: 736: 733: 730: 726: 725: 719: 716: 713: 710: 706: 705: 699: 696: 693: 690: 686: 685: 682: 679: 676: 673: 669: 668: 665: 662: 659: 656: 648:Stevens Levels 630: 629: 622: 615: 608: 607: 606: 574: 571: 570: 569: 563: 557: 546: 543: 538: 535: 521: 518: 497: 494: 485:Main article: 479:Main article: 468: 465: 459: 456: 431: 428: 423: 420: 414: 411: 394: 391: 374:Main article: 362: 359: 353: 350: 326:Main article: 315: 312: 288:Main article: 276: 273: 250:Main article: 247: 244: 239:Main article: 236: 233: 211: 208: 182: 179: 173: 170: 117:graphic design 82:Jacques Bertin 49: 46: 37:Jacques Bertin 29:graphic design 15: 13: 10: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 1232: 1221: 1218: 1216: 1213: 1211: 1208: 1207: 1205: 1192: 1186: 1183: 1179: 1173: 1170: 1166: 1160: 1157: 1154: 1150: 1144: 1141: 1137: 1131: 1128: 1124: 1118: 1115: 1111: 1105: 1102: 1098: 1094: 1088: 1086: 1082: 1078: 1072: 1069: 1066: 1061: 1058: 1055: 1051: 1046: 1043: 1040: 1034: 1031: 1028: 1024: 1019: 1016: 1011: 1007: 1003: 999: 993: 990: 986: 980: 978: 974: 970: 964: 962: 958: 954: 948: 946: 944: 942: 940: 936: 932: 926: 923: 919: 913: 911: 907: 903: 897: 894: 890: 884: 881: 878: 874: 870: 864: 862: 860: 858: 856: 852: 848: 844: 838: 836: 832: 825: 823: 821: 816: 810: 808: 804: 798: 790: 783: 779: 777: 774: 771: 768: 767: 764: 760: 757: 754: 751: 748: 747: 743: 740: 737: 734: 731: 728: 727: 723: 720: 717: 714: 711: 708: 707: 704: 700: 697: 694: 691: 688: 687: 683: 680: 677: 674: 671: 670: 666: 663: 660: 657: 654: 653: 649: 644: 643: 639: 635: 634:symbolization 626: 623: 619: 616: 612: 609: 605:dissociative. 603: 600:In contrast, 599: 598: 595: 592: 591: 590: 588: 584: 580: 572: 567: 564: 561: 558: 555: 552: 551: 550: 544: 542: 536: 534: 532: 527: 519: 517: 515: 512:, then chose 511: 507: 503: 495: 493: 488: 482: 473: 466: 464: 457: 455: 453: 449: 445: 441: 437: 429: 427: 421: 419: 412: 410: 408: 404: 400: 393:Grain/Spacing 392: 390: 387: 383: 377: 367: 360: 358: 351: 349: 346: 344: 340: 339:figure-ground 335: 329: 320: 313: 311: 309: 305: 302:, especially 301: 300:thematic maps 297: 291: 281: 274: 272: 270: 266: 262: 258: 253: 245: 242: 234: 232: 230: 226: 216: 209: 207: 205: 199: 192: 187: 180: 178: 171: 169: 165: 163: 159: 155: 151: 147: 146:How Maps Work 142: 137: 132: 130: 126: 125:Joel Morrison 122: 118: 114: 110: 106: 102: 98: 94: 89: 87: 83: 79: 75: 71: 67: 63: 59: 55: 47: 45: 42: 38: 34: 30: 26: 22: 1185: 1177: 1172: 1164: 1159: 1148: 1143: 1135: 1130: 1122: 1117: 1109: 1104: 1096: 1076: 1071: 1060: 1053: 1045: 1033: 1026: 1018: 1010:the original 1001: 992: 984: 968: 952: 930: 925: 917: 901: 896: 888: 883: 872: 846: 842: 811: 805:and a clear 800: 689:Hierarchical 631: 624: 617: 610: 601: 593: 579:Image schema 576: 565: 559: 553: 548: 540: 523: 513: 509: 501: 499: 490: 461: 433: 425: 416: 406: 402: 398: 396: 385: 381: 379: 355: 347: 333: 331: 328:Colorfulness 295: 293: 268: 264: 260: 256: 255: 229:transit maps 221: 200: 196: 175: 166: 162:Human vision 145: 135: 133: 112: 108: 101:Human vision 90: 85: 73: 51: 40: 20: 18: 1210:Cartography 784:of terrain 742:Temperature 735:Color value 701:Languages, 658:Distinction 444:scatterplot 436:coordinates 413:Arrangement 403:granularity 352:Orientation 70:cartography 1204:Categories 826:References 797:Map symbol 526:pixelating 520:Resolution 334:saturation 246:Color: hue 695:Color Hue 667:Examples 514:crispness 290:Lightness 204:cartogram 191:cartogram 150:Semiotics 141:Semiotics 97:semiotics 769:Cyclical 729:Interval 502:Elements 430:Position 405:or just 284:purpose. 1023:"Shape" 744:, Year 709:Ordinal 672:Nominal 452:isoline 386:pattern 382:texture 370:Africa. 48:History 782:Aspect 537:Height 448:fields 440:charts 103:, and 31:, and 749:Ratio 712:Order 655:Level 628:size. 566:Sound 510:focus 407:grain 399:grain 296:value 265:green 235:Color 210:Shape 23:, in 341:and 332:The 269:blue 181:Size 119:and 64:and 56:and 54:maps 384:or 261:red 257:Hue 252:Hue 156:), 1206:: 1095:, 1084:^ 1052:, 1025:, 1004:. 1000:. 976:^ 960:^ 938:^ 909:^ 854:^ 834:^ 589:. 533:. 267:, 263:, 189:A 160:, 99:, 27:, 19:A

Index

cartographic design
graphic design
data visualization
Jacques Bertin
maps
statistical charts
William Playfair
Charles Joseph Minard
cartography
Arthur H. Robinson
Jacques Bertin
École pratique des hautes études
semiotics
Human vision
Gestalt psychology
graphic design
information visualization
Joel Morrison
cartographic generalization
Semiotics
Semiotics
Semiotic theory of Charles Sanders Peirce
Gestalt psychology
Human vision

cartogram
cartogram

Cartographic generalization
transit maps

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.

↑