Knowledge (XXG)

Well travelled road effect

Source đź“ť

28:
destination, when one candidate route includes a familiar route, whilst the other candidate route includes no familiar routes. The effect is most salient when subjects are driving, but is still detectable for pedestrians and users of public transport. The effect has been observed for centuries but was first studied scientifically in the 1980s and 1990s following from earlier "heuristics and biases" work undertaken by
27:
in which travellers will estimate the time taken to traverse routes differently depending on their familiarity with the route. Frequently travelled routes are assessed as taking a shorter time than unfamiliar routes. This effect creates errors when estimating the most efficient route to an unfamiliar
43:, it is hypothesised that drivers use less cognitive effort when traversing familiar routes and therefore underestimate the time taken to traverse the familiar route. The well travelled road effect has been hypothesised as a reason that self-reported 165:
Jackson, W. Burke; Jucker, James V. (1982). "An Empirical Study of Travel Time Variability and Travel Choice Behavior".
260: 66: 44: 61: 56: 71: 125: 236: 209: 140: 201: 174: 104: 227:
Rubia, K; Smith, A (2004). "The neural correlates of cognitive time management: a review".
29: 192:
Zakay, Dan; Fallach, Eli (1984). "Immediate and remote time estimation — A comparison".
24: 254: 205: 126:"Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: an executive-control perspective" 33: 40: 240: 144: 213: 178: 109: 92: 148: 8: 108: 83: 7: 229:Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis 133:Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis 14: 97:Perception & Psychophysics 1: 206:10.1016/0001-6918(84)90054-4 124:Zakay, D; Block, RA (2004). 91:Allan, Lorraine G. (1979). 277: 21:well travelled road effect 93:"The perception of time" 67:List of cognitive biases 45:experience curve effects 167:Transportation Science 62:Availability heuristic 57:Accuracy and precision 179:10.1287/trsc.16.4.460 47:are overestimated. 110:10.3758/BF03204158 194:Acta Psychologica 72:Weber–Fechner law 268: 261:Cognitive biases 245: 244: 224: 218: 217: 189: 183: 182: 162: 156: 155: 153: 147:. Archived from 130: 121: 115: 114: 112: 88: 276: 275: 271: 270: 269: 267: 266: 265: 251: 250: 249: 248: 226: 225: 221: 191: 190: 186: 164: 163: 159: 151: 128: 123: 122: 118: 90: 89: 85: 80: 53: 30:Daniel Kahneman 17: 12: 11: 5: 274: 272: 264: 263: 253: 252: 247: 246: 219: 184: 173:(4): 460–475. 157: 154:on 2016-05-24. 116: 103:(5): 340–354. 82: 81: 79: 76: 75: 74: 69: 64: 59: 52: 49: 39:Much like the 25:cognitive bias 16:Cognitive bias 15: 13: 10: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 273: 262: 259: 258: 256: 242: 238: 235:(3): 329–40. 234: 230: 223: 220: 215: 211: 207: 203: 199: 195: 188: 185: 180: 176: 172: 168: 161: 158: 150: 146: 142: 139:(3): 319–28. 138: 134: 127: 120: 117: 111: 106: 102: 98: 94: 87: 84: 77: 73: 70: 68: 65: 63: 60: 58: 55: 54: 50: 48: 46: 42: 37: 35: 31: 26: 22: 232: 228: 222: 200:(1): 69–81. 197: 193: 187: 170: 166: 160: 149:the original 136: 132: 119: 100: 96: 86: 38: 34:Amos Tversky 20: 18: 41:Stroop task 78:References 255:Category 241:15283476 145:15283475 51:See also 214:6507128 239:  212:  143:  152:(PDF) 129:(PDF) 23:is a 237:PMID 210:PMID 141:PMID 32:and 19:The 202:doi 175:doi 105:doi 257:: 233:64 231:. 208:. 198:57 196:. 171:16 169:. 137:64 135:. 131:. 101:26 99:. 95:. 36:. 243:. 216:. 204:: 181:. 177:: 113:. 107::

Index

cognitive bias
Daniel Kahneman
Amos Tversky
Stroop task
experience curve effects
Accuracy and precision
Availability heuristic
List of cognitive biases
Weber–Fechner law
"The perception of time"
doi
10.3758/BF03204158
"Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: an executive-control perspective"
PMID
15283475
the original
doi
10.1287/trsc.16.4.460
doi
10.1016/0001-6918(84)90054-4
PMID
6507128
PMID
15283476
Category
Cognitive biases

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.

↑