Knowledge (XXG)

Ethogram

Source 📝

45:, which in the ethogram is given a descriptive name such as "head forward" or "chest-beating display", and not "head forward threat" or "chest-beating threat". This degree of objectivity is required because what looks like "courtship" might have a completely different function, and in addition, the same motor patterns in different species can have very different functions (e.g. tail wagging in cats and dogs). Objectivity and clarity in the definitions of behaviours also improve 1048: 52:
Often, ethograms are hierarchical in presentation. The defined behaviours are recorded under broader categories of behaviour which may allow functional inference such that "head forward" is recorded under "Aggression". In ethograms of social behaviour, the ethogram may also indicate the "Giver" and
56:
Sometimes, the definition of a behaviour in an ethogram may have arbitrary components. For example, "Stereotyped licking" might be defined as "licking the bars of the cage more than 5 times in 30 seconds". The definition may be arguable, but if it is stated clearly, it fulfils the requirements of
60:
Some ethograms are given in pictorial form and not only catalogue the behaviours but indicate the frequency of their occurrence and the probability that one behaviour follows another. This probability can be indicated numerically or by the thickness of an arrow connecting the two behaviours.
111:
Ethograms have also been applied to research concerning the behavioural response of animals to the presence of humans. For example, it has been used to analyze the reactions of black bears and baboons to humans.
413:
Brunberg, Emma; Wallenbeck, Anna; Keeling, Linda J. (August 2011). "Tail biting in fattening pigs: Associations between frequency of tail biting and other abnormal behaviours".
247: 982: 521:
Jordan, Robert H.; Burghardt, Gordon M. (1986-01-12). "Employing an Ethogram to Detect Reactivity of Black Bears (Ursus americanus) to the Presence of Humans".
608: 987: 262: 335:
Anderson, Claes; Arun, Attur Shanmugam; Jensen, Per (2010). "Habituation to environmental enrichment in captive sloth bears--effect on stereotypies".
84: 633: 440:
Dawkins, Marian Stamp (August 1989). "Time budgets in Red Junglefowl as a baseline for the assessment of welfare in domestic fowl".
206:"Behavioural observations of singly-housed grey short-tailed opossums (Monodelphis domestica) in standard and enriched environments" 103:), departures from the ethogram of ancestral species and the behaviour of captive animals upon release into a natural environment. 83:
Ethograms are used extensively in the study of welfare science. Ethograms can be used to detect the occurrence or prevalence of
370:
Sherwin, CM; Richards, GJ; Nicol, CJ (August 2010). "Comparison of the welfare of layer hens in 4 housing systems in the UK".
691: 467:
McBride, G.; Parer, I.P.; Foenander, F. (January 1969). "The Social Organization and Behaviour of the Feral Domestic Fowl".
1077: 1025: 721: 100: 803: 1004: 896: 753: 210: 748: 1018: 701: 31: 676: 270: 46: 494:
Stolba, A.; Wood-Gush, D. G. M. (2 September 2010). "The behaviour of pigs in a semi-natural environment".
835: 798: 788: 733: 681: 626: 88: 78: 558:"The influence of observer presence on baboon (Papio spp.) and rhesus macaque (Macaca mulatta) behavior" 241: 951: 941: 706: 666: 661: 175: 1032: 813: 96: 946: 738: 716: 696: 395: 317: 151: 1072: 1051: 758: 726: 619: 587: 538: 387: 352: 229: 1011: 956: 823: 656: 577: 569: 530: 503: 476: 449: 422: 379: 344: 307: 299: 219: 184: 143: 129: 966: 906: 891: 876: 861: 830: 783: 92: 38:
and functional inference as to their possible purpose. For example, a species may use a
931: 886: 881: 763: 671: 582: 557: 534: 42: 480: 303: 61:
Sometimes the proportion of time that each behaviour occupies can be represented in a
1066: 936: 926: 921: 916: 845: 778: 686: 453: 321: 171:"A Proposed Ethogram of Large-Carnivore Predatory Behavior, Exemplified by the Wolf" 155: 23:
is a catalogue or inventory of behaviours or actions exhibited by an animal used in
961: 911: 871: 840: 793: 573: 426: 399: 35: 383: 901: 866: 30:
The behaviours in an ethogram are usually defined to be mutually exclusive and
507: 189: 170: 542: 66: 62: 39: 591: 391: 356: 233: 224: 205: 773: 768: 642: 134: 24: 808: 147: 348: 312: 57:
scientific repeatability and clarity of reporting and data recording.
556:
Iredale, Steven K.; Nevill, Christian H.; Lutz, Corrine K. (2010).
711: 290:
Mason, Georgia J. (June 1991). "Stereotypies: a critical review".
132:(1964). "Mouse mutants studied by means of ethological methods". 818: 615: 204:
Wilkinson, M., Stirton, C. and McConnachie, A. (October 2010).
169:
MacNulty, Daniel R.; Mech, L. David; Smith, Douglas W. (2007).
611: 996: 975: 854: 649: 16:Catalogue of behaviours exhibited by an animal 983:Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour 627: 8: 246:: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list ( 988:International Society for Applied Ethology 634: 620: 612: 581: 311: 223: 188: 607:A sample ethogram of general behaviour: 121: 271:The University of Tennessee, Knoxville 239: 107:Reactions of animals to human presence 7: 535:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1986.tb01002.x 14: 1047: 1046: 562:Applied Animal Behaviour Science 442:Applied Animal Behaviour Science 415:Applied Animal Behaviour Science 692:Bee learning and communication 574:10.1016/j.applanim.2009.11.002 427:10.1016/j.applanim.2011.04.019 1: 481:10.1016/S0066-1856(69)80003-8 304:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80640-2 454:10.1016/0168-1591(89)90126-3 384:10.1080/00071668.2010.502518 469:Animal Behaviour Monographs 99:), normal behaviours (e.g. 1094: 76: 53:"Receiver" of activities. 47:inter-observer reliability 1042: 749:Evolutionary neuroscience 508:10.1017/S0003356100040411 190:10.1644/06-MAMM-A-119R1.1 702:Behavioral endocrinology 897:Irenäus Eibl-Eibesfeldt 677:Animal sexual behaviour 372:British Poultry Science 836:Tool use by non-humans 789:Philosophical ethology 734:Comparative psychology 682:Animal welfare science 225:10.1258/la.2010.010040 79:Animal welfare science 73:Animal welfare science 942:William Homan Thorpe 707:Behavioural genetics 667:Animal consciousness 662:Animal communication 176:Journal of Mammalogy 1078:Behavioral concepts 697:Behavioural ecology 85:abnormal behaviours 1026:Behavioral Ecology 947:Nikolaas Tinbergen 739:Emotion in animals 717:Cognitive ethology 267:Greenberg Homepage 211:Laboratory Animals 148:10.1007/BF01664181 101:comfort behaviours 1060: 1059: 952:Jakob von Uexküll 722:Comfort behaviour 496:Animal Production 349:10.1002/zoo.20301 263:"Anolis ethogram" 1085: 1050: 1049: 1012:Animal Cognition 1005:Animal Behaviour 957:Wolfgang Wickler 657:Animal cognition 636: 629: 622: 613: 596: 595: 585: 553: 547: 546: 518: 512: 511: 491: 485: 484: 464: 458: 457: 437: 431: 430: 410: 404: 403: 367: 361: 360: 332: 326: 325: 315: 298:(6): 1015–1037. 292:Animal Behaviour 287: 281: 280: 278: 277: 258: 252: 251: 245: 237: 227: 201: 195: 194: 192: 166: 160: 159: 126: 1093: 1092: 1088: 1087: 1086: 1084: 1083: 1082: 1063: 1062: 1061: 1056: 1038: 992: 971: 967:Solly Zuckerman 907:Karl von Frisch 892:Richard Dawkins 877:John B. Calhoun 862:Patrick Bateson 850: 784:Pain in animals 645: 640: 605: 600: 599: 555: 554: 550: 520: 519: 515: 493: 492: 488: 466: 465: 461: 439: 438: 434: 412: 411: 407: 369: 368: 364: 334: 333: 329: 289: 288: 284: 275: 273: 260: 259: 255: 238: 203: 202: 198: 168: 167: 163: 130:Abeelen, J.H.F. 128: 127: 123: 118: 109: 93:feather pecking 81: 75: 17: 12: 11: 5: 1091: 1089: 1081: 1080: 1075: 1065: 1064: 1058: 1057: 1055: 1054: 1043: 1040: 1039: 1037: 1036: 1029: 1022: 1019:Animal Welfare 1015: 1008: 1000: 998: 994: 993: 991: 990: 985: 979: 977: 973: 972: 970: 969: 964: 959: 954: 949: 944: 939: 934: 932:Desmond Morris 929: 924: 919: 914: 909: 904: 899: 894: 889: 887:Marian Dawkins 884: 882:Charles Darwin 879: 874: 869: 864: 858: 856: 852: 851: 849: 848: 843: 838: 833: 828: 827: 826: 821: 816: 811: 801: 796: 791: 786: 781: 776: 771: 766: 764:Human ethology 761: 756: 751: 746: 741: 736: 731: 730: 729: 719: 714: 709: 704: 699: 694: 689: 684: 679: 674: 672:Animal culture 669: 664: 659: 653: 651: 647: 646: 641: 639: 638: 631: 624: 616: 604: 603:External links 601: 598: 597: 548: 513: 502:(2): 419–425. 486: 459: 432: 421:(1–2): 18–25. 405: 362: 327: 282: 261:Greenberg, N. 253: 196: 183:(3): 595–605. 161: 120: 119: 117: 114: 108: 105: 74: 71: 43:threat display 15: 13: 10: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 1090: 1079: 1076: 1074: 1071: 1070: 1068: 1053: 1045: 1044: 1041: 1035: 1034: 1030: 1028: 1027: 1023: 1021: 1020: 1016: 1014: 1013: 1009: 1007: 1006: 1002: 1001: 999: 995: 989: 986: 984: 981: 980: 978: 974: 968: 965: 963: 960: 958: 955: 953: 950: 948: 945: 943: 940: 938: 937:Thomas Sebeok 935: 933: 930: 928: 927:Konrad Lorenz 925: 923: 922:Julian Huxley 920: 918: 917:Heini Hediger 915: 913: 910: 908: 905: 903: 900: 898: 895: 893: 890: 888: 885: 883: 880: 878: 875: 873: 870: 868: 865: 863: 860: 859: 857: 853: 847: 846:Zoomusicology 844: 842: 839: 837: 834: 832: 829: 825: 822: 820: 817: 815: 812: 810: 807: 806: 805: 802: 800: 797: 795: 792: 790: 787: 785: 782: 780: 779:Neuroethology 777: 775: 772: 770: 767: 765: 762: 760: 757: 755: 752: 750: 747: 745: 742: 740: 737: 735: 732: 728: 725: 724: 723: 720: 718: 715: 713: 710: 708: 705: 703: 700: 698: 695: 693: 690: 688: 687:Anthrozoology 685: 683: 680: 678: 675: 673: 670: 668: 665: 663: 660: 658: 655: 654: 652: 648: 644: 637: 632: 630: 625: 623: 618: 617: 614: 610: 609: 602: 593: 589: 584: 579: 575: 571: 567: 563: 559: 552: 549: 544: 540: 536: 532: 529:(2): 89–115. 528: 524: 517: 514: 509: 505: 501: 497: 490: 487: 482: 478: 474: 470: 463: 460: 455: 451: 447: 443: 436: 433: 428: 424: 420: 416: 409: 406: 401: 397: 393: 389: 385: 381: 378:(4): 488–99. 377: 373: 366: 363: 358: 354: 350: 346: 343:(6): 705–14. 342: 338: 331: 328: 323: 319: 314: 309: 305: 301: 297: 293: 286: 283: 272: 268: 264: 257: 254: 249: 243: 235: 231: 226: 221: 217: 213: 212: 207: 200: 197: 191: 186: 182: 178: 177: 172: 165: 162: 157: 153: 149: 145: 141: 137: 136: 131: 125: 122: 115: 113: 106: 104: 102: 98: 94: 90: 86: 80: 72: 70: 68: 64: 58: 54: 50: 48: 44: 41: 37: 33: 28: 26: 22: 1031: 1024: 1017: 1010: 1003: 962:E. O. Wilson 912:Jane Goodall 872:Donald Broom 841:Zoosemiotics 794:Sociobiology 743: 606: 568:(1): 53–57. 565: 561: 551: 526: 522: 516: 499: 495: 489: 472: 468: 462: 448:(1): 77–80. 445: 441: 435: 418: 414: 408: 375: 371: 365: 340: 336: 330: 295: 291: 285: 274:. Retrieved 266: 256: 242:cite journal 218:(4): 364–9. 215: 209: 199: 180: 174: 164: 139: 133: 124: 110: 89:stereotypies 82: 59: 55: 51: 36:subjectivity 29: 20: 18: 902:Dian Fossey 867:Marc Bekoff 855:Ethologists 475:: 125–181. 337:Zoo Biology 97:tail-biting 34:, avoiding 1067:Categories 804:Structures 799:Stereotypy 313:10214/4622 276:2011-03-18 116:References 77:See also: 1033:Behaviour 976:Societies 814:Honeycomb 543:1439-0310 142:: 79–94. 67:bar chart 63:pie chart 32:objective 1073:Ethology 1052:Category 997:Journals 824:Instinct 774:Learning 769:Instinct 744:Ethogram 727:Grooming 650:Branches 643:Ethology 592:20228948 523:Ethology 392:20924842 357:20069629 322:53187334 234:20807718 156:44915853 135:Genetica 40:putative 25:ethology 21:ethogram 754:Feeding 583:2836517 400:8968010 590:  580:  541:  398:  390:  355:  320:  232:  154:  87:(e.g. 831:Swarm 759:Hover 712:Breed 396:S2CID 318:S2CID 152:S2CID 819:Nest 809:Hive 588:PMID 539:ISSN 388:PMID 353:PMID 248:link 230:PMID 578:PMC 570:doi 566:122 531:doi 504:doi 477:doi 450:doi 423:doi 419:133 380:doi 345:doi 308:hdl 300:doi 220:doi 185:doi 144:doi 65:or 49:. 27:. 19:An 1069:: 586:. 576:. 564:. 560:. 537:. 527:73 525:. 500:48 498:. 471:. 446:24 444:. 417:. 394:. 386:. 376:51 374:. 351:. 341:29 339:. 316:. 306:. 296:41 294:. 269:. 265:. 244:}} 240:{{ 228:. 216:44 214:. 208:. 181:88 179:. 173:. 150:. 140:34 138:. 95:, 91:, 69:. 635:e 628:t 621:v 594:. 572:: 545:. 533:: 510:. 506:: 483:. 479:: 473:2 456:. 452:: 429:. 425:: 402:. 382:: 359:. 347:: 324:. 310:: 302:: 279:. 250:) 236:. 222:: 193:. 187:: 158:. 146::

Index

ethology
objective
subjectivity
putative
threat display
inter-observer reliability
pie chart
bar chart
Animal welfare science
abnormal behaviours
stereotypies
feather pecking
tail-biting
comfort behaviours
Abeelen, J.H.F.
Genetica
doi
10.1007/BF01664181
S2CID
44915853
"A Proposed Ethogram of Large-Carnivore Predatory Behavior, Exemplified by the Wolf"
Journal of Mammalogy
doi
10.1644/06-MAMM-A-119R1.1
"Behavioural observations of singly-housed grey short-tailed opossums (Monodelphis domestica) in standard and enriched environments"
Laboratory Animals
doi
10.1258/la.2010.010040
PMID
20807718

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.