Knowledge (XXG)

Socioemotional selectivity theory

Source πŸ“

27:) is a life-span theory of motivation. The theory maintains that as time horizons shrink, as they typically do with age, people become increasingly selective, investing greater resources in emotionally meaningful goals and activities. According to the theory, motivational shifts also influence cognitive processing. Aging is associated with a relative preference for positive over negative information in individuals who have had rewarding relationships. This selective narrowing of social interaction maximizes positive emotional experiences and minimizes emotional risks as individuals become older. According to this theory, older adults systematically hone their social networks so that available social partners satisfy their emotional needs. 86:– older adults report better control of their emotions and fewer negative emotions than do younger adults. At the same time, culture seems to color how aging-related effects impact one's emotional life: Whereas older Americans were shown to de-emphasize negative experiences more than younger Americans, no such effect has been observed in Japan. Instead, older Japanese were shown to assign a greater value to positive aspects of otherwise negative experiences than younger Japanese, whereas no such effect has been observed in the U.S. 58:
does not reveal any information about one’s health, personality, cognitive development, role in social life, and social status. Evaluating old age within the framework of various biological, psychological, and sociological factors makes it easier for researchers to obtain more detailed and specific results. Regardless of the characteristics selected to assess old age, it is possible to say that aging has biological, physiological, psychological, and sociological impacts on individuals, either positive or negative.
117:
negative information and more likely to be distorted in a positive direction. This version of the positivity effect was coined by Laura L. Carstensen's research team. There is a debate about the cross-cultural generalizability of the aging-related positivity effect, with some evidence for different types of emotional processing among Americans as compared to Japanese.
50:," in which an individual, usually a young and physically healthy individual, unreasonably believes (either consciously or unconsciously) that their time horizons are more limited than they actually are, with the effect that the individual undervalues long-term goals and long-run pleasure and instead disproportionately pursues short-term goals and pleasure, thereby 139:
actively engage the mPFC differently from younger adults, which in turn yields diverging amygdala activation patterns. The opposite pattern was observed for words. Although older adults showed a positivity effect in memory for words, they did not display one for pictures. Thus, the positivity effect may arise from ageing differences in MPFC use during encoding.
116:
also refers to age differences in emotional attention and memory. As people get older, they experience fewer negative emotions and they tend to look to the past in a positive light. In addition, compared with younger adults' memories, older adults' memories are more likely to consist of positive than
34:
When people perceive their future as open ended, they tend to focus on future-oriented and development- or knowledge-related goals, but when they feel that time is running out and the opportunity to reap rewards from future-oriented goals' realization is dwindling, their focus tends to shift towards
133:
However, the positivity effect may be different for stimuli processed automatically (pictures) and stimuli processed in a more controlled manner (words). Compared to words, pictures tend to be processed more rapidly and they engage emotion processing centres earlier. Automatic stimuli are processed
30:
The theory also focuses on the types of goals that individuals are motivated to achieve. Knowledge-related goals aim at knowledge acquisition, career planning, the development of new social relationships and other endeavors that will pay off in the future. Emotion-related goals are aimed at emotion
57:
The thought of nearing β€œthe end” leads to an increased orientation toward social and emotional targets. Here, β€œthe end” signifies death, which is generally associated with chronological old age. However, it would be inaccurate to limit old age to chronological aging because one’s chronological age
138:
and dorsal MPFC, whereas controlled stimuli are processed in the temporal pole and ventral MPFC. Compared to younger adults, older adults showed less amygdala activation and more MPFC activation for negative than positive pictures. Increased motivation to regulate emotion leads older adults to
125:
One theory of the positivity effect in older adults' memories is that it is produced by cognitive control mechanisms that improve and decrease negative information due to older adults' greater focus on emotional regulation. Research shows an age-related reversal in the valence of information
103:
and eye-tracking methods). However, the effect also differs across cultures. For example, Hong Kong Chinese looked away from happy stimuli and more towards fearful stimuli, and the difference in attention pattern was related to differences in self-construal.
39:, young adulthood vs. old adulthood), but the shift in goal priorities is a gradual process that begins in early adulthood. Importantly, the theory contends that the cause of these goal shifts is not age itself, 142:
Recent study by Helene Fung's group in China University in Hong Kong and Deep Longevity utilized artificial intelligence to show that people who are unhappy and lonely have accelerated biological age.
697:
Lockenhoff, Corinna E.; Carstensen, Laura L. (2004). "Socioemotional Selectivity Theory, Aging, and Health: The Increasingly Delicate Balance Between Regulating Emotions and Making Tough Choices".
344:
Fung, H. H.; Lu, A. Y.; Goren, D.; Isaacowitz, D. M.; Wadlinger; Wilson, H. R. (2008). "Age-related positivity enhancement is not universal: older Chinese look away from positive stimuli".
736:
Fung, Helene H.; Carstensen, Laura L. (2004). "Motivational Changes in Response to Blocked Goals and Foreshortened Time: Testing Alternatives to Socioemotional Selectivity Theory".
379:
Fung, H. H.; Isaacowitz, D. M.; Lu, A. Y.; Li, T. (2010). "Interdependent self-construal moderates the age-related negativity reduction effect in memory and visual attention".
130:(MPFC). In younger adults, more MPFC activity was found in the presence of negative stimuli compared to positive stimuli whereas in older adults this was reversed. 99:
Studies have found that older adults are more likely than younger adults to pay more attention to positive than to negative stimuli (as assessed by the
31:
regulation, the pursuit of emotionally gratifying interactions with social partners and other pursuits whose benefits can be realized in the present.
309:
Grossmann, Igor; Karasawa, Mayumi; Kan, Chiemi; Kitayama, Shinobu (2014). "A cultural perspective on emotional experiences across the life span".
587:"Optimizing future well-being with artificial intelligence: self-organizing maps (SOMs) for the identification of islands of emotional stability" 422: 765:
Pruzan, Katherine; Isaacowitz, Derek M. (2006). "An Attentional Application of Socioemotional Selectivity Theory in College Students".
668:
Carstensen, Laura L.; Isaacowitz, Derek M.; Charles, Susan T. (1999). "Taking time seriously: A theory of socioemotional selectivity".
876: 282: 881: 786:
Carstensen, Laura L.; Mikels, Joseph A. (2005). "At the Intersection of Emotion and Cognition. Aging and the Positivity Effect".
542:
Leclerc, C.; Kensinger, E. (2011). "Neural processing of emotional pictures and words: A comparison of young and older adults".
198: 495:"Effects of aging on functional connectivity of the amygdala during negative evaluation: A network analysis of fMRI data" 647:
Carstensen, L. L. (1992). "Motivation for social contact across the life span: A theory of socioemotional selectivity".
901: 906: 46:
This justified shift in perspective is the rational equivalent of the psychological perceptual disorder known as "
127: 35:
present-oriented and emotion- or pleasure-related goals. Research on this theory often compares age groups (
47: 795: 706: 449: 800: 877:
https://web.archive.org/web/20060926170243/http://www.nrpa.org/content/default.aspx?documentId=1974
711: 24: 585:
Galkin, Fedor; Kochetov, Kirill; Keller, Michelle; Zhavoronkov, Alex; Etcoff, Nancy (2022-06-20).
813: 567: 288: 100: 215: 858: 753: 724: 685: 656: 626: 608: 559: 524: 475: 396: 361: 326: 278: 243: 235: 194: 161: 83: 886: 848: 838: 805: 774: 745: 716: 677: 616: 598: 551: 514: 506: 465: 457: 388: 353: 318: 270: 227: 151: 79: 75: 67: 51: 156: 43:, not the passage of time itself, but rather an age-associated shift in time perspective. 510: 453: 853: 826: 621: 586: 519: 494: 470: 437: 895: 809: 778: 720: 292: 817: 571: 71: 555: 216:"Influence of time on social preferences: Implications for life-span development" 681: 357: 231: 749: 843: 612: 239: 461: 862: 757: 728: 689: 630: 563: 528: 479: 400: 365: 330: 247: 660: 603: 135: 392: 322: 262: 274: 66:
Researchers have found that across diverse samples – ranging from
261:
HARORLI, Emre (2020-10-15). Aydin, Hatice; Kurnaz, Aysel (eds.).
412: 410: 214:
Fung, Helene H.; Carstensen, Laura L.; Lutz, Amy M. (1999).
54:
and often even actively reducing their long-term prospects.
438:"The Influence of a Sense of Time on Human Development" 887:
List of papers related to aging and positivity effect
827:"The Theory Behind the Age-Related Positivity Effect" 421:
sfn error: no target: CITEREFMatherCarstensen2005 (
52:diverting resources from investment for the future 882:Positivity effect and cognitive control in aging 416: 493:St. Jacques, P; Dolcos, F; Cabeza, R (2010). 8: 788:Current Directions in Psychological Science 304: 302: 267:Current Perspectives on Consumer Psychology 191:A Topical Approach to Life-Span Development 184: 182: 180: 178: 176: 852: 842: 799: 710: 620: 602: 518: 469: 825:Reed, A. E.; Carstensen, L. L. (2012). 172: 23:; developed by Stanford psychologist 7: 511:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.03.012 263:"Socioemotional Selectivity Theory" 14: 90:Positivity effect in older adults 17:Socioemotional selectivity theory 810:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00348.x 779:10.1046/j.1467-9507.2006.00344.x 721:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00301.x 649:Nebraska Symposium on Motivation 1: 544:Developmental Neuropsychology 193:. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. 556:10.1080/87565641.2010.549864 417:Mather & Carstensen 2005 923: 682:10.1037/0003-066X.54.3.165 436:Carstensen, L. L. (2006). 358:10.1037/0882-7974.23.2.440 232:10.1037/0882-7974.14.4.595 189:Santrock, John W. (2002). 750:10.1037/0882-7974.19.1.68 844:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00339 128:medial prefrontal cortex 62:Cross-cultural incidence 831:Frontiers in Psychology 462:10.1126/science.1127488 699:Journal of Personality 670:American Psychologist 604:10.18632/aging.204061 499:Neurobiology of Aging 126:processed within the 738:Psychology and Aging 381:Psychology and Aging 346:Psychology and Aging 220:Psychology and Aging 48:foreshortened future 454:2006Sci...312.1913C 448:(5782): 1913–1915. 121:Hypothesized causes 25:Laura L. Carstensen 902:Theories of ageing 767:Social Development 101:dot-probe paradigm 84:European-Americans 907:Social psychology 597:(12): 4935–4958. 162:Positivity effect 114:positivity effect 80:Chinese Americans 76:African-Americans 914: 866: 856: 846: 821: 803: 782: 761: 732: 714: 693: 664: 635: 634: 624: 606: 582: 576: 575: 539: 533: 532: 522: 490: 484: 483: 473: 433: 427: 426: 414: 405: 404: 393:10.1037/a0019079 376: 370: 369: 341: 335: 334: 323:10.1037/a0036041 306: 297: 296: 269:. Peter Lang D. 258: 252: 251: 211: 205: 204: 186: 152:Aging and memory 922: 921: 917: 916: 915: 913: 912: 911: 892: 891: 873: 824: 801:10.1.1.551.1460 785: 764: 735: 705:(6): 1395–424. 696: 667: 646: 643: 638: 584: 583: 579: 541: 540: 536: 492: 491: 487: 435: 434: 430: 420: 415: 408: 378: 377: 373: 343: 342: 338: 308: 307: 300: 285: 260: 259: 255: 213: 212: 208: 201: 188: 187: 174: 170: 157:Negativity bias 148: 123: 110: 97: 92: 64: 12: 11: 5: 920: 918: 910: 909: 904: 894: 893: 890: 889: 884: 879: 872: 871:External links 869: 868: 867: 822: 783: 762: 733: 712:10.1.1.319.121 694: 665: 642: 639: 637: 636: 577: 550:(4): 519–538. 534: 505:(2): 315–327. 485: 428: 406: 371: 336: 298: 283: 275:10.3726/b17423 253: 226:(4): 595–604. 206: 199: 171: 169: 166: 165: 164: 159: 154: 147: 144: 122: 119: 109: 106: 96: 93: 91: 88: 63: 60: 13: 10: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 919: 908: 905: 903: 900: 899: 897: 888: 885: 883: 880: 878: 875: 874: 870: 864: 860: 855: 850: 845: 840: 836: 832: 828: 823: 819: 815: 811: 807: 802: 797: 793: 789: 784: 780: 776: 772: 768: 763: 759: 755: 751: 747: 743: 739: 734: 730: 726: 722: 718: 713: 708: 704: 700: 695: 691: 687: 683: 679: 676:(3): 165–81. 675: 671: 666: 662: 658: 654: 650: 645: 644: 640: 632: 628: 623: 618: 614: 610: 605: 600: 596: 592: 588: 581: 578: 573: 569: 565: 561: 557: 553: 549: 545: 538: 535: 530: 526: 521: 516: 512: 508: 504: 500: 496: 489: 486: 481: 477: 472: 467: 463: 459: 455: 451: 447: 443: 439: 432: 429: 424: 418: 413: 411: 407: 402: 398: 394: 390: 386: 382: 375: 372: 367: 363: 359: 355: 351: 347: 340: 337: 332: 328: 324: 320: 317:(4): 679–92. 316: 312: 305: 303: 299: 294: 290: 286: 284:9783631832240 280: 276: 272: 268: 264: 257: 254: 249: 245: 241: 237: 233: 229: 225: 221: 217: 210: 207: 202: 196: 192: 185: 183: 181: 179: 177: 173: 167: 163: 160: 158: 155: 153: 150: 149: 145: 143: 140: 137: 131: 129: 120: 118: 115: 107: 105: 102: 95:In perception 94: 89: 87: 85: 81: 77: 73: 72:Catholic nuns 69: 61: 59: 55: 53: 49: 44: 42: 38: 32: 28: 26: 22: 18: 834: 830: 791: 787: 770: 766: 744:(1): 68–78. 741: 737: 702: 698: 673: 669: 652: 648: 594: 590: 580: 547: 543: 537: 502: 498: 488: 445: 441: 431: 387:(2): 321–9. 384: 380: 374: 352:(2): 440–6. 349: 345: 339: 314: 310: 266: 256: 223: 219: 209: 190: 141: 132: 124: 113: 111: 98: 65: 56: 45: 40: 36: 33: 29: 20: 16: 15: 896:Categories 794:(3): 117. 773:(2): 326. 655:: 209–54. 200:0072435992 168:References 68:Norwegians 796:CiteSeerX 707:CiteSeerX 613:1945-4589 293:241881389 240:1939-1498 112:The term 108:In recall 863:23060825 818:59067078 758:15065932 729:15509287 690:10199217 631:35723468 564:21516546 529:18455837 480:16809530 401:20545417 366:18573017 331:24749641 248:10632147 146:See also 136:amygdala 854:3459016 837:: 339. 661:1340521 641:Sources 622:9271294 572:6300556 520:3541693 471:2790864 450:Bibcode 442:Science 311:Emotion 134:in the 861:  851:  816:  798:  756:  727:  709:  688:  659:  629:  619:  611:  570:  562:  527:  517:  478:  468:  399:  364:  329:  291:  281:  246:  238:  197:  814:S2CID 591:Aging 568:S2CID 289:S2CID 859:PMID 754:PMID 725:PMID 686:PMID 657:PMID 627:PMID 609:ISSN 560:PMID 525:PMID 476:PMID 423:help 397:PMID 362:PMID 327:PMID 279:ISBN 244:PMID 236:ISSN 195:ISBN 41:i.e. 37:e.g. 849:PMC 839:doi 806:doi 775:doi 746:doi 717:doi 678:doi 617:PMC 599:doi 552:doi 515:PMC 507:doi 466:PMC 458:doi 446:312 389:doi 354:doi 319:doi 271:doi 228:doi 82:to 78:to 74:to 70:to 21:SST 898:: 857:. 847:. 833:. 829:. 812:. 804:. 792:14 790:. 771:15 769:. 752:. 742:19 740:. 723:. 715:. 703:72 701:. 684:. 674:54 672:. 653:40 651:. 625:. 615:. 607:. 595:14 593:. 589:. 566:. 558:. 548:36 546:. 523:. 513:. 503:31 501:. 497:. 474:. 464:. 456:. 444:. 440:. 409:^ 395:. 385:25 383:. 360:. 350:23 348:. 325:. 315:14 313:. 301:^ 287:. 277:. 265:. 242:. 234:. 224:14 222:. 218:. 175:^ 865:. 841:: 835:3 820:. 808:: 781:. 777:: 760:. 748:: 731:. 719:: 692:. 680:: 663:. 633:. 601:: 574:. 554:: 531:. 509:: 482:. 460:: 452:: 425:) 419:. 403:. 391:: 368:. 356:: 333:. 321:: 295:. 273:: 250:. 230:: 203:. 19:(

Index

Laura L. Carstensen
foreshortened future
diverting resources from investment for the future
Norwegians
Catholic nuns
African-Americans
Chinese Americans
European-Americans
dot-probe paradigm
medial prefrontal cortex
amygdala
Aging and memory
Negativity bias
Positivity effect





ISBN
0072435992
"Influence of time on social preferences: Implications for life-span development"
doi
10.1037/0882-7974.14.4.595
ISSN
1939-1498
PMID
10632147
"Socioemotional Selectivity Theory"
doi

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.

↑