Knowledge (XXG)

Blocking (linguistics)

Source 📝

117:, who stated that the more restricted of two competing rules would have precedence within a language system. During the 1960s, this insight was reformulated as the so-called "elsewhere principle", used in the language of several contemporary theories of grammar. Hermann Paul, a German linguist, wrangled with the idea, proposing an alternative theory that accounts for the crucial role of frequency in how blocking can be learned. 324: 31:
phenomenon in which a possible form for a word cannot surface because it is "blocked" by another form whose features are the most appropriate to the surface form's environment. More basically, it may also be construed as the "non-occurrence of one form due to the simple existence of another."
104:
features create slots or cells in which items can appear. Blocking happens when one cell is engaged by one form as opposed to another. Blocking has been explained along two primary dimensions: the size of the blocking object, and the existence of ungrammatical forms.
81:
Blocking may also prevent the formation of words with existing synonyms, particularly if the blocked form is morphologically complex and the existing synonym is morphologically simple, e.g. *
365: 277: 297: 168: 143: 389: 193: 384: 358: 218: 351: 28: 233: 93: 331: 238: 293: 289: 251: 199: 189: 164: 139: 335: 285: 243: 35: 114: 378: 113:
Blocking was first described in the 5th or 4th century BC by the Indian grammarian
20: 323: 186:
Morphological productivity : structural constraints in English derivation
255: 247: 203: 101: 78:, which in this case inherits features from an older morphological process. 97: 138:. Beijing: Foreign Language Teaching and Research Press. p. 71. 39: 74:), because it is "blocked" by the presence of the competing form 136:
Longman Dictionary of Language Teaching and Applied Linguistics
66:). This plural marker is not, however, acceptable on the word 339: 92:
One possible approach to blocking effects is that of
359: 134:Richards, Jack C.; et al., eds. (2005). 85:which is blocked by the existing simple form 8: 217:Embick, David; Marantz, Alec (Winter 2008). 282:Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics 366: 352: 237: 290:10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.001.0001 126: 7: 320: 318: 161:Word Formation in Generative Grammar 338:. You can help Knowledge (XXG) by 14: 322: 38:employs processes such as the 1: 188:. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 163:. Cambridge, MA.: MIT Press. 276:Rainer, Franz (2016-06-09), 16:Phenomenon in word formation 390:Linguistic morphology stubs 219:"Architecture and Blocking" 406: 317: 248:10.1162/ling.2008.39.1.1 159:Aronoff, Mark (1976). 94:distributed morphology 385:Linguistic morphology 332:linguistic morphology 96:, which asserts that 184:Ingo, Plag (1999). 226:Linguistic Inquiry 347: 346: 299:978-0-19-938465-5 170:978-0-262-51017-2 145:978-7-5600-4882-6 397: 368: 361: 354: 326: 319: 309: 308: 307: 306: 273: 267: 266: 264: 262: 241: 223: 214: 208: 207: 181: 175: 174: 156: 150: 149: 131: 405: 404: 400: 399: 398: 396: 395: 394: 375: 374: 373: 372: 315: 313: 312: 304: 302: 300: 275: 274: 270: 260: 258: 239:10.1.1.717.8216 221: 216: 215: 211: 196: 183: 182: 178: 171: 158: 157: 153: 146: 133: 132: 128: 123: 111: 17: 12: 11: 5: 403: 401: 393: 392: 387: 377: 376: 371: 370: 363: 356: 348: 345: 344: 327: 311: 310: 298: 268: 209: 195:978-3110158335 194: 176: 169: 151: 144: 125: 124: 122: 119: 110: 107: 36:Word formation 15: 13: 10: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 402: 391: 388: 386: 383: 382: 380: 369: 364: 362: 357: 355: 350: 349: 343: 341: 337: 334:article is a 333: 328: 325: 321: 316: 301: 295: 291: 287: 283: 279: 272: 269: 261:September 18, 257: 253: 249: 245: 240: 235: 231: 227: 220: 213: 210: 205: 201: 197: 191: 187: 180: 177: 172: 166: 162: 155: 152: 147: 141: 137: 130: 127: 120: 118: 116: 108: 106: 103: 99: 95: 90: 88: 84: 79: 77: 73: 69: 65: 61: 57: 53: 49: 45: 41: 40:plural marker 37: 33: 30: 29:morphological 26: 22: 340:expanding it 329: 314: 303:, retrieved 281: 271: 259:. Retrieved 229: 225: 212: 185: 179: 160: 154: 135: 129: 112: 91: 86: 82: 80: 75: 71: 67: 63: 59: 55: 51: 47: 43: 34: 24: 18: 232:(1): 1–53. 42:in English 21:linguistics 379:Categories 305:2023-09-28 278:"Blocking" 121:References 256:1530-9150 234:CiteSeerX 102:syntactic 204:40311603 98:semantic 76:children 70:(as in * 25:blocking 109:History 83:stealer 27:is the 296:  254:  236:  202:  192:  167:  142:  115:Pāṇini 72:childs 64:wishes 50:(e.g. 330:This 222:(PDF) 87:thief 68:child 336:stub 294:ISBN 263:2011 252:ISSN 200:OCLC 190:ISBN 165:ISBN 140:ISBN 100:and 62:and 60:wish 56:dogs 54:and 286:doi 244:doi 58:or 52:dog 46:or 23:, 19:In 381:: 292:, 284:, 280:, 250:. 242:. 230:39 228:. 224:. 198:. 89:. 48:es 367:e 360:t 353:v 342:. 288:: 265:. 246:: 206:. 173:. 148:. 44:s

Index

linguistics
morphological
Word formation
plural marker
distributed morphology
semantic
syntactic
Pāṇini
ISBN
978-7-5600-4882-6
ISBN
978-0-262-51017-2
ISBN
978-3110158335
OCLC
40311603
"Architecture and Blocking"
CiteSeerX
10.1.1.717.8216
doi
10.1162/ling.2008.39.1.1
ISSN
1530-9150
"Blocking"
doi
10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.001.0001
ISBN
978-0-19-938465-5
Stub icon
linguistic morphology

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.